SMS: social and cognitive impact

Authors

  • Elena Alonso Universitat de València Spain
  • Manuel Perea Universitat de València Spain

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.24310/espsiescpsi.v2i1.13355

Keywords:

Linguistics, reading, eye movements, phonology, SMS language

Abstract

The growing use of mobile text messages (hereinafter SMS) has given place to a debate on the social impact and the psychological effects of this new mass media. In the recent years the massive use of mobiles and Internet has generated a new way of communication based on the abbreviated use of language. In this work we examine the role of SMS as an effective tool of communication in the way of establishing social relationships among young individuals. Furthermore we examine the reading cognitive cost of SMS; and derived from this analysis and the content of the SMS sentences we extract two conclusions. The first one is that SMS language is economical enough to transfer relevant information using the abbreviated language even though it implies a reading cost, the second conclusion is that SMS language implies a socio-cultural aspect worthy to be reflected on.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Metrics

Metrics Loading ...

References

Acha, J. y Perea, M. (2008). The effect of neighborhood frequency in reading: Evidence with transposed- letter neighbors. Cognition, 108, 290-300.

Berent, I. y Perfetti, C.A. (1995). A rose is a REEZ: The two cycles model of phonology assembly in reading English. Psychological Review, 102, 146-184.

Brown, G. y Yule, G. (1983). Discourse analysis. Cambridge: Cambrige University Press.

Carreiras, M., Ferrand, L., Grainger, J. y Perea, M. (2005). Sequential effects of phonological priming in visual word recognition. Psychological Science, 16, 585-589.

Carreiras, M., Gillon-Dowens, M., Vergara, M. y Perea, M. (en prensa). Are vowels and consonants processed differently? ERP evidence with a delayed letter paradigm. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

Carreiras, M. y Price, C.J. (2008). Brain activation for consonants and vowels. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1727-1735.

Crystal, D. (2001). Language and the Internet. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Chandler, D. (1994). The Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis. Höflich, J. y Rössler, P. (2002). Más que un teléfono: El teléfono móvil y el uso del SMS por parte de los adolescentes alemanes. Resultados de un estudio piloto. Estudios de Juventud, 57, 79-99.

Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 71-99.

Jakobson, R. (1963). Essais de linguistique générale. Paris: Editions de Minuit.

Kul, M. (2007). Phonology in text messages. Pozna Studies in Contemporary Linguistics, 43, 43–57.

Lee, H.-W., Rayner, K. y Pollatsek, A. (2001). The relative contribution of consonants and vowels to word identification during reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 189-192.

Levi-Strauss, C. (1949). Les structures élémentaires de la parenté. Paris: Mouton.

López-Rúa, P. (2007). Teaching L2 vocabulary through SMS language: Some didactic guidelines. Elia: Estudios de lingu?ística inglesa aplicada, 7, 165-188.

Mehler, J., Peña, M., Nespor, M. y Bonatti, L.L. (2006). The “soul” of language does not use statistics: Reflections on vowels and consonants. Cortex, 42, 846-854.

Moles, A. A. (1967). Sociodynamique de la Culture. Paris: Mouton.

Muñoz-Rivas, M. J. y Agustín, S. (2005). La adicción al teléfono móvil. Psicología Conductual, 13, 481-493.

Perea, M., Acha, J. y Carreiras, M. (2008). Eye movements when reading text messaging (txt msgng). Enviado para publicación.

Perea, M. y Pollatsek, A. (1998). The effects of neighborhood frequency in reading and lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 767-777.

Pinker, S. (1995). The language instinct. New York: Harper-Collins.

Pollatsek, A., Lesch, M., Morris, R.K. y Rayner, K. (1992). Phonological codes are used to integrate information across saccades in word recognition and reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 148-162.

Pollatsek, A., Perea, M. y Carreiras, M. (2005). Does conal prime CANAL more than cinal? Masked phonological priming effects in Spanish with the lexical decision task. Memory and Cognition, 33, 557- 565.

Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372-422.

Reid, D. y Reid, F. (2007). Insights into the Social and Psychological Effects of SMS Text Messaging. University of Plymouth. Disponible en: http://www.160characters.org/documents/SocialEffectsofTextMessaging.pdf

Saussure, de, F. (1959). Course in general linguistics. New York: The Philosophical Library.

Sapir, E. (1963). Selected Writings of Edward Sapir in Language, Culture and Personality. Los Angeles: University of California Press.

Schramm, W. (1963). The science of human communication. New York: Basic Books.

Slattery, T.J., Pollatsek, A., y Rayner, A. (2007). The time course of phonological and orthographic processing of acronyms in reading: Evidence from eye movements. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 412-417.

Tagliamonte, S.A. y Denis, D. (2008). Linguistic ruin? lol! Instant messaging and teen language. American Speech, 83, 3-34.

Tapscott, D. (1998). Growing Up Digital: The Rise of the Net Generation. New York: McGraw Hill.

White, S.J. (2008). Eye movement control during reading: Effects of word frequency and orthographic familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 205-223.

Whorf, B.L. (1956). Language, Thought, and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Cambridge: MIT Press.

Published

2008-12-31

How to Cite

Alonso, E., & Perea, M. (2008). SMS: social and cognitive impact. Escritos De Psicología - Psychological Writings, 2(1), 24–31. https://doi.org/10.24310/espsiescpsi.v2i1.13355

Issue

Section

Reports