Dyslexia subtypes in languages differing in orthographic transparency: English, French and Spanish
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.24310/espsiescpsi.v4i2.13314Keywords:
Developmental Dyslexia, Subtypes, Orthographic Depth, Phonological Dyslexia, Surface Dyslexia, Mixed Profile, Word-level reading skills, Vocal Response LatencyAbstract
The existence of dissociated profiles in developmental dyslexia (the phonological profile with a selective deficit of the phonological reading route, and the surface profile with a selective deficit of the lexical reading route) versus mixed profiles (with both deficits) remains a major theoretical and clinical issue, along with the prevalence of these profiles and the variation in that prevalence across languages with different degrees of orthographic transparency. These issues are examined in a review of studies conducted in English, French and Spanish in which subtyping was established us-ing either the classical method or the regression method. The most reliable results were those obtained with the classical method: (1) the most prevalent profile is the mixed profile; (2) the prevalence of dissociated profiles differs across languages and measures, phonological profiles being more frequent in the accuracy-based English studies than in the accuracy-based French and Spanish studies, and less frequent in the accuracy-based than in the latency-based French and Spanish studies. These last findings probably reflect a measurement issue, as it is easy to use the phonological reading route in transparent orthographies: in these cases, reading speed must be used to detect phonological deficits. These results are not consistent with the idea that clear-cut subtypes can be detected in developmental dyslexia.
Downloads
Metrics
Publication Facts
Reviewer profiles N/A
Author statements
Indexed in
-
—
- Academic society
- N/A
- Publisher
- UNIVERSIDAD DE MÁLAGA
References
Backman, J., Bruck, M., Hebert, M. & Seidenberg, M. S. (1984). Acquisition and use of spelling sound correspondences in reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 114-133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(84)90022-5
Bailey, C. E., Manis, F. R., Pedersen, W. C. & Seidenberg, M. S. (2003). Variations among developmental dyslexics: Evidence from a printed-word-learning task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 125-154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2003.10.004
Bates, E., Appelbaum, M., Salcedo, J., Saygin, A.P. & Pizzamiglio, L. (2003). Quantifying Dissociations in Neuropsychological Research. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 11281153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/jcen.25.8.1128.16724
Bowey, J. A. & Rutherford, J. (2007). Imbalanced word-reading profiles in eighth-graders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 169-196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2006.11.001
Bryant, P. & Impey, L. (1986). The similarities between normal readers and developmental and acquired dyslexics. Cognition, 24, 121-137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(86)90007-7
Castles, A. & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47, 149-180. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)90003-E
Castles, A., Datta, H., Gayan, J. & Olson, R. K. (1999). Varieties of reading disorder: Genetic and environmental influences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 73-94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1998.2482
Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P. & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual route and parallel processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100, 589-608. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.100.4.589
Coltheart, M., Masterson, J., Byng, S., Prior, M. & Riddoch, J. (1983). Surface dyslexia. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35, 469-595.
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R. & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204-256. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204
Crawford, J. R. & Garthwaite, P. H. (2005). Evaluation of criteria for classical dissociations in single-case studies by Monte Carlo simulation. Neuropsychology, 19, 664-678. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.19.5.664
Crawford, J. R., Garthwaite, P. H. & Gray, C. D. (2003). Wanted: Fully operational definitions of dissociations in single-case studies. Cortex, 39, 357-370. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70117-5
Davies, R., Cuetos, F. & Glez-Seijas, R.M. (2007). Reading development and dyslexia in a transparent orthography: A survey of Spanish children. Annals of Dyslexia, 57, 179-198. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11881-007-0010-1
Génard, N., Mousty, P., Content, A., Alegria, J., Leybaert, J. & Morais, J. (1998). Methods to establish subtypes of developmental dyslexia. In P. Reitsma & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Problems and interventions in literacy development (pp. 163-176). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Goswami, U., Gombert, J. E. & Barrera, L. F. (1998). Children’s orthographic representations and linguistic transparency: Nonsense word reading in English, French and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 19-52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400010560
Harm, M. W. & Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review, 106, 491-528. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.3.491
Jiménez, J. E. & Hernández-Valle, I. (2000). Word identification and reading disorders in the Spanish language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 267-275.
Jiménez, J. E., Rodríguez, C. & Ramírez, G. (2009). Spanish developmental dyslexia: Prevalence, cognitive profile and home literacy experiences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 103, 167-185. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.004
Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E. & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 1-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11881-003-0001-9
Manis, F. R. & Bailey, C. E. (2008). Exploring heterogeneity in developmental dyslexia: A longitudinal investigation. In G. Reid, A.J. Fawcett, L. S. Siegel & F. Manis (Eds.), The Sage Handbook of Dyslexia (pp. 149-173). London: Sage Publications Ltd.
Manis, F. R., Custodio, R. & Szeszulski, P. A. (1993). Development of phonologic and orthographic skills: A 2-year longitudinal study of dyslexic children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 64-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1993.1026
Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., Doi, L. M., McBride-Chang, C. & Peterson, A. (1996). On the basis of two subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 58, 157-195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(95)00679-6
Martin, J., Colé, P., Leuwers, C., Casalis, S., Zorman, M. & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2010). Reading in French-speaking adults with dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 60, 238-264. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11881-010-0043-8
Olson, R. K., Forsberg, H., Wise, B. & Rack, J. (1994). Measurement of word recognition, orthographic and phonological skills. In G. R. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues (pp. 243-275). Baltimore/London: Paul H. Brookes.
Paulesu, E., Démonet, J.-F., Fazio, F., McCrory, E., Chanoine, V., Brunswick, N, Cappa, S. F., Cossu, G., Habib, M., Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. (2001). Dyslexia, Cultural diversity and Biological unity. Science, 291, 2165-2167. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1057179
Peereman, R. & Content, A. (1998). Quantitative analysis of orthography to phonology mapping in English and French (on-line). http://homepages.vub.ac.be/~acontent/OPMapping.html.
Peereman, R., Lété, B. & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2007). Manulex-infra: Distributional characteristics of infra-lexical and lexical units in child-directed written material. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 39, 593-603.
Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J. & Olson, R. K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 29-53. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/747832
Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: Specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 212-218. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00035-7
Serrano, F.D. & Defior, S. (2008). Dyslexia speed problems in a transparent orthography. Annals of Dyslexia, 58, 81-95.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11881-008-0013-6
Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151-218. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2
Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentrism of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier orthography”. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584-615. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584
Shaywitz, S.E. & Shaywitz, B.A. (2005). Dyslexia (Specific Reading Disability). Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1301-1309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.043
Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2003). Reading acquisition: Cross linguistic data. In T.Nunes, & P.Bryant (Eds.), Handbook of children’s literacy (pp. 43-66). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., Béchennec, D. & Kipffer-Piquard, A. (2005). French normative data on reading and related skills from EVALEC, a new computerized battery of tests (end Grade 1, Grade 2, Grade 3, and Grade 4). European Review of Applied Psychology, 55, 157-186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2004.11.002
Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., Kipffer-Piquard, A., Pinton, F. & Billard, C. (2009). Reliability and prevalence of an atypical development of phonological skills in French-speaking dyslexics. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 811-842.
Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., Lacert, P. & Serniclaes, W. (2000). On subtypes of developmental dyslexia: Evidence from processing time and accuracy scores. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 88-104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0087332
Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P. & Serniclaes, W. (2006). Reading acquisition and developmental dyslexia. Essays in developmental psychology. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L. S., Béchennec, D. & Serniclaes, W. (2003). Development of phonological and orthographic processing in reading aloud, in silent reading and in spelling: A four year longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 194-217. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00024-9
Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L. S., Jiménez, J. E. & Ziegler, J. C. (2011). Prevalence and reliability of phonological, surface, and mixed profiles in dyslexia: A review of studies conducted in languages varying in orthographic depth. Scientific Studies of Reading.
Stanovich, K. E (1999). The sociopsychometrics of learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 350-361. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221949903200408
Stanovich, K. E. & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24-53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.86.1.24
Stanovich, K. E., Siegel, L. S. & Gottardo, A. (1997). Converging evidence for phonological and surface subtypes of reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 114-127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.1.114
Temple, C. M. & Marshall, J. C. (1983). A case study of developmental phonological dyslexia. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 517-533. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1983.tb01883.x
Valdois, S., Bosse, M. L., Ans, B., Carbonnel, S., Zorman, M., David, D. & Pellat, J. (2003). Phonological and visual processing deficits can dissociate in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from two case studies. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 541-572. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1025501406971
Zabell, C. & Everatt, J. (2002). Surface and phonological subtypes of adult developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia, 8, 160-177. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dys.223
Ziegler, J. C., Castel, C., Pech-Georgel, C., George, F., Alario, F.X. & Perry, C. (2008). Developmental dyslexia and the Dual Route Model of reading: Simulating Individual differences and subtypes. Cognition, 107, 151–178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.09.004
Ziegler, J. & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3-29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
All contents published in Escritos de Psicología are protected under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-SA 4.0) license. All about this license is available in the following link: <http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0>
Users can copy, use, redistribute, share and exhibit publicly as long as:
- The original source and authorship of the material are cited (Journal, Publisher and URL of the work).
- It is not used for comercial purposes.
- The existence of the license and its especifications are mentioned.
There are two sets of authors’ rights: moral and property rights. Moral rights are perpetual prerogatives, unrenounceable, not-transferable, unalienable, imprescriptible and inembargable. According to authors’ rights legislation, Escritos de Psicología recognizes and respects authors moral rights, as well as the ownership of property rights. The property rights are referred to the benefits that are gained by the use or the dissemination of works. Escritos de Psicología is published in an open access form and it is exclusively licenced by any means for doing or authorising distribution, dissemination, reproduction, , adaptation, translation or arrangement of works.
Authors are responsable for obtaining the necessary permission to use copyrighted images.