What is needed for learning multiplication? The role of basic numerical skills and anxiety
Keywords:
mathematics anxiety, arithmetic, basic numerical skills, longitudinal, primary educationAbstract
The mastering of single-digit multiplication is a challenge for many children. Recent research has suggested that some basic numerical skills, such as those measured in Arabic numeral comparison tasks (i.e. access to numerical representations from symbols), are good predictors of children’s performance on single-digit multiplication. On the other hand, emotional variables, such as math anxiety, play a role in learning arithmetic facts. The aim of the current study was to study the role of several basic numerical cognitive skills and emotional factors in single-digit multiplication solving in a longitudinal study including school-age children. Math anxiety, trait anxiety, non-symbolic numerical comparison, Arabic numeral comparison, and subtraction fluency were assessed in first-, second-, and third-grade Spanish children, (N = 141; boys = 66, girls = 75; age range: 6 – 9 years). We assessed multiplication fluency 3 years later. The results showed that performance on Arabic numeral comparison and subtraction tasks significantly predicted performance on multiplication tasks. This finding supports previously published results. Regarding anxiety factors, it was also found that the participant’s score in trait anxiety, but not math anxiety, was a marginally significant predictor. The results highlight the relevance of access to numerical semantic codes from symbolic representations and fluency in combining these representations in subtraction in predicting children’s performance in single-digit multiplication. Anxiety factors seem to be of less relevance.
Downloads
Metrics
References
2. Ashcraft, M. H. (2002). Math anxiety: Personal, educational, and cognitive consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 181–185. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00196
3. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd Edition). New York: Academic Press.
4. Defever, E., Sasanguie, D., Gebuis, T., & Reynvoet, B. (2011). Children’s representation of symbolic and non-symbolic magnitude examined with the priming paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 174–186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.01.002
5. Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44, 1–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(92)90049-n
6. De Smedt, B., Noël, M. P., Gilmore, C., & Ansari, D. (2013). How do symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude processing skills relate to individual differences in children’s mathematical skills? A review of evidence from brain and behavior. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 2, 48–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2013.06.001
7. De Smedt, B., Verschaffel, L., & Ghesquière, P. (2009). The predictive value of numerical magnitude comparison for individual differences in mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 469–479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.010
8. Geary, D. C. (2011). Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in mathematics: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1539-1552. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025510
9. Gebuis, T., & Reynvoet, B. (2012). The interplay between no symbolic number and its continuous visual properties. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 642-648. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026218
10. Gross, J., Hudson, C., & Price, D. (2009). The Long-Term Costs of Numeracy Difficulties (Every Child a Chance Trust and KPMG, London).
11. Hecht, S. A. (1999). Individual solution processes while solving addition and multiplication math facts in adults. Memory & Cognition, 27, 1097-1107. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03201239
12. Hopko, D.R., Mahadevan, R., Bare, R.L., & Hunt, M. (2003). The Abbreviated Math Anxiety Scale (AMAS). Construction, validity and reliability. Assessment, 10, 178-182. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191103010002008
13. Inglis, M., Attridge, N., Batchelor, S., & Gilmore, C. (2011). Non-verbal number acuity correlates with symbolic mathematics achievement: But only in children. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 1222–1229. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0154-1
14. Kilpatrick, J., Swafford, J., & Findell, B. (2001). Adding it up: Helping children learn mathematics. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
15. Krinzinger, H., Kaufmann, L., & Willmes, K. (2009). Math Anxiety and Math Ability in Early Primary School Years. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 27, 206-225. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282908330583
16. Kutner, M.H., Nachtsheim, C.J. & Neter, J. (2004). Applied Linear Regression Models (4th Edition). Chicago: McGraw-Hill
17. LeFevre, J. A., Bisanz, J., Daley, K. E., Buffone, L., Greenham, S. L., & Sadesky, G. S. (1996). Multiple routes to solution of single digit multiplication problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 284-306. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.125.3.284
18. LeFevre, J. A., Fast, L., Skwarchuk, S. L., Smith-Chant, B. L., Bisanz, J., Kamawar, D., et al. (2010). Pathways to mathematics: Longitudinal predictors of performance. Child Development, 81, 1753–1767. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01508.x
19. Libertus, M.E., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011). Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability. Developmental Science, 14, 1292–1300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01080.x
20. Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2013). Is approximate number precision a stable predictor of math ability? Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 126–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.02.001
21. Lyons, I. M., & Ansari, D. (2015). Foundations of Children’s Numerical and Mathematical Skills: The Roles of Symbolic and Non-symbolic Representations of Numerical Magnitude. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 48, 93–116. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.acdb.2014.11.003
22. Lyons, I. M., & Beilock, S. L. (2011). Numerical ordering ability mediates the relation between number-sense and arithmetic competence. Cognition, 121, 256–261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.07.009
23. Lyons, I. M., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). When Math Hurts: Math Anxiety Predicts Pain Network Activation in Anticipation of Doing Math. PLoS ONE, 7, e48076. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048076
24. Lyons, I. M., Price, G. R., Vaessen, A., Blomert, L., & Ansari, D. (2014). Numerical predictors of arithmetic success in grades 1-6. Developmental Science, 17, 714–726. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12152
25. McLeod, D.B. (1994). Research on affect and mathematics learning in the JRME: 1970 to the present. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 25, 637-647. https://doi.org/10.2307/749576
26. OCDE (2010). The High Cost of Low Educational Performance. The Long-run Economic Impact of Improving Educational Outcomes. London: OECD.
27. Parsons, S., & Bynner, J. (2005). Measuring basic skills for longitudinal study. NRDC Report, October, www.nrdc.org.uk.
28. Ramirez, G., Chang, H., Maloney, E. A., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2016). On the relationship between math anxiety and math achievement in early elementary school: The role of problem-solving strategies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 83–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.014
29. Richardson, F.C., & Suinn, R.M. (1972). The Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 19, 551–554. https://doi.org/10.1037/t02345-000
30. Romero, S. G., Rickard, T. C., & Bourne, L. E. (2006). Verification of multiplication facts: An investigation using retrospective protocols. American Journal of Psychology, 119, 87-121. https://doi.org/10.2307/20445320
31. Roussel, J. L., Fayol, M., & Barrouillet, P. (2002). From procedural computation to direct retrieval. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 61-104.
32. Sasanguie, D., De Smedt, B., Defever, E., & Reynvoet, B. (2011). Association between basic numerical abilities and mathematics achievement. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 344–357. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835x.2011.02048.x
33. Sasanguie, D., Göbel, S. M., Moll, K., Smets, K., & Reynvoet, B. (2013). Approximate number sense, symbolic number processing, or number-space mappings: What underlies mathematics achievement? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 418–431. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.012
34. Soltész, F., Szucs, D., & Szucs, L. (2010). Relationships between magnitude representation, counting and memory in 4- to 7-year-old children: A developmental study. Behavioural and Brain Functions, 6, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-6-13
35. Tejedor, B., Santos, M.A., García-Orza, J., Carratalà, P., & Navas, M. (2009). Variables explicativas de la ansiedad frente a las matemáticas: Un estudio de una muestra de 6° de primaria. Anuario de Psicología, 40, 345–355.
36. Thomas, G., & Dowker, A. (2000). Mathematics anxiety and related factors in young children. Paper presented at British Psychological Society Developmental Section Conference; Bristol, UK.
37. Wang, Z., Lukowski, S. L., Hart, S. A., Lyons, I. M., Thompson, L. A., Kovas, & Petrill, S. A. (2015). Is Math Anxiety Always Bad for Math Learning? The Role of Math Motivation. Psychological Science, 26, 1863–1876. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615602471
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
All contents published in Escritos de Psicología are protected under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-SA 4.0) license. All about this license is available in the following link: <http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0>
Users can copy, use, redistribute, share and exhibit publicly as long as:
- The original source and authorship of the material are cited (Journal, Publisher and URL of the work).
- It is not used for comercial purposes.
- The existence of the license and its especifications are mentioned.
There are two sets of authors’ rights: moral and property rights. Moral rights are perpetual prerogatives, unrenounceable, not-transferable, unalienable, imprescriptible and inembargable. According to authors’ rights legislation, Escritos de Psicología recognizes and respects authors moral rights, as well as the ownership of property rights. The property rights are referred to the benefits that are gained by the use or the dissemination of works. Escritos de Psicología is published in an open access form and it is exclusively licenced by any means for doing or authorising distribution, dissemination, reproduction, , adaptation, translation or arrangement of works.
Authors are responsable for obtaining the necessary permission to use copyrighted images.