The Relationship Between Perceived Emotional Intelligence and Risk Behaviour in the Setting of Health

Authors

  • María Teresa Sánchez López Universidad de Málaga, España Spain
  • Alberto Megías Robles Universidad de Málaga, España Spain
  • Raquel Gómez Leal Universidad de Málaga, España Spain
  • María José Gutiérrez Cobo Universidad de Málaga, España Spain
  • Pablo Fernández Berrocal Universidad de Málaga, España Spain

Keywords:

Emotional intelligence, TMMS, risk behaviour, health, DOSPERT-30

Abstract

The aim of the present study was to investigate the association between perceived emotional intelligence and the probability of engaging in risk behaviour in the setting of health. The study sample comprised 219 participants aged between 18 and 59 years. Their perceived emotional intelligence skills were assessed using the Trait-Meta-Mood Scale (TMMS), and their probability of involvement in health-related risk behaviour was assessed using the Domain-Specific Risk-Taking Scale (DOSPERT-30). The results show that emotional clarity and repair skills were inversely associated with risk behaviour. Furthermore, a direct association was found between emotional attention and clarity skills, and between emotional clarity and repair. The age of the participants was inversely associated with engaging in risk behaviour. Finally, gender differences were found in emotional attention (the female participants had higher levels of attention to emotions). In conclusion, perceived emotional intelligence skills (clarity and repair) can act as protective factors against risk-taking behaviour that threatens health and physical safety, such as having unprotected sex, drunk driving, obesity, and so on. We discuss the clinical implications of training in these skills.

 

Key words: Emotional intelligence; TMMS; risk behaviour; health; DOSPERT-30.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Metrics

Metrics Loading ...

References

Alessi, S. M., y Petry, N. M. (2003). Pathological gambling severity is associated with impulsivity in a delay discounting procedure. Behavioural Processes, 64, 345–354. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0376-6357(03)00150-5

Amlung, M., Petker, T., Jackson, J., Balodis, I., y MacKillop, J. (2016). Steep discounting of delayed monetary and food rewards in obesity: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 46, 2423–2434. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291716000866

Baker, F., Johnson, M. W., y Bickel, W. K. (2003). Delay discounting in current and never-before cigarette smokers: Similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and magnitude. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 382–392. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.112.3.382

Baumeister, R., Vohs, K., Nathan DeWall, C., y Zhang, L. (2007). How emotion shapes behavior: Feedback, anticipation, and reflection, rather than direct causation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 167-203. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868307301033

Bickel, W. K., Odum, A. L., y Madden, G. J. (1999). Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: Delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146, 447– 454. https://doi.org/10.1007/pl00005490

Blais, A. R., y Weber, E. U. (2006). A domain-specific risk-taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judgment and Decision Making, 1 (1), 33-47.

Brackett, M. A., Mayer, J. D., y Warner, R. M. (2004). Emotional intelligence and its relation to everyday behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1387-1402. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0191-8869(03)00236-8

Brackett, M. A., Patti, J., Stern, R., Rivers, S. E., Elbertson, N., Chisholm, C. y Salovey, P. (2009). A sustainable, skill–based model to building emotionally literate schools. En R. Thompson, M. Hughes y J. B. Terrell (eds.), Handbook of developing emotional and social intelligence: Best practices, case studies, and tools (pp. 329–358). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Cabello, R., Castillo, R., Rueda, P., y Fernández-Berrocal, P., (2016). Programa INTEMO+. Mejorar la inteligencia emocional de los adolescentes. Madrid: Pirámide.

Castillo-Gualda, R., Cabello, R., Herrero, M., Rodríguez-Carvajal, R., y Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2017). A three-year emotional intelligence intervention to reduce adolescent aggression: the mediating role of unpleasant affectivity. J. Res. Adolesc. 28, 186–198. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12325

Daugherty, J. R., y Brase, G. L. (2010). Taking time to be healthy: Predicting health behaviors with delay discounting and time perspective. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 202–207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.10.007

Dixon, M. R., Marley, J., y Jacobs, E. A. (2003). Delay discounting by pathological gamblers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 449–458. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2003.36-449

Ekman, P. (1973). Darwin and facial expression: A century of research in review. Oxford, England: Academic Press.

Fernández-Berrocal, P. y Extremera, N. (2008). A review of Trait Meta-Mood Research. En A.M. Columbus (ed.), Advances in psychology research: Vol. 55. (pp.17-45). New York: Nova Science Publishers.

Feldman-Barret, B. L., Gross, J. J., Conner, T. C., y Benvenuto, M. (2001). Knowing what you’re feeling and knowing what to do about it: Mapping the relation between emotion differentiation and emotion regulation. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 713-724. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000239

Fernández-Berrocal, P., Extremera, N., y Ramos, N. (2004). Validity and reliability of the Spanish modified version of the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. Psychological Reports, 94, 751-755. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.94.3.751-755

Fernández-Berrocal, P., Ramos, N., y Extremera, N. (2001). Inteligencia Emocional, supresión crónica de pensamientos y ajuste psicológico. Boletín de Psicología, 70, 79-95.

Fields, S. A., Sabet, M., Peal, A., y Reynolds, B. (2011). Relationship between weight status and delay discounting in a sample of adolescent cigarette smokers. Behavioural Pharmacology, 22, 266–268. https://doi.org/10.1097/fbp.0b013e328345c855

Gray, J. C., y MacKillop, J. (2015). Impulsive delayed reward discounting as a genetically-influenced target for drug abuse prevention: A critical evaluation. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1104. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01104

Gross, J. J., y John, O. P. (2002). Wise emotion regulation. En L. Feldman Barrett and P. Salovey (Eds.), The wisdom of feelings: Psychological processes in emotional intelligence (pp. 297-318). New York: Guilford.

Green, L., y Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 769–792. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.769

Heil, S. H., Johnson, M. W., Higgins, S. T., y Bickel, W. K. (2006). Delay discounting in currently using and currently abstinent cocaine-dependent outpatients and non-drug-using matched controls. Addictive Behaviors, 31, 1290–1294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.09.005

Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., y Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52 (6), 1122–1131. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12254

Izard, C. E., Fine, S., Mostow, A., Trentacosta, C., y Campbell, J. (2002). Emotion processes in normal and abnormal development and preventive intervention. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 761-787. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579402004066

Joseph, D. L., y Newman, D. A. (2010). Emotional intelligence: An integrative meta-analysis and cascading model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 54–78. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017286

Keltner, D., y Haidt, J. (2001). Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. En W. Parrott (Ed.), Emotions in social psychology: Essential readings (pp. 175-184). New York, NY: Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999399379168

Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., y Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 78–87. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.128.1.78

Kirby, K. N., y Petry, N. M. (2004). Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drugusing controls. Addiction, 99, 461–471. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2003.00669.x

Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Lozano, L. M., Megías, A., Catena, A., Perales, J. C., Baltruschat, S., y Cándido, A. (2017). Spanish validation of the Domain-Specific Risk-Taking (DOSPERT-30) Scale. Psicothema, 29 (1), 111–118. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2016.132

MacKillop, J., Miranda, R., Jr., Monti, P. M., Ray, L. A., Murphy, J. G., Rohsenow, D. J., y Gwaltney, C. J. (2010). Alcohol demand, delayed reward discounting, and craving in relation to drinking and alcohol use disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 106–114. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017513

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., y Caruso, D. R. (2002). Mayer-Salovey-Caruso emotional intelligence test (MSCEIT) user’s manual.

Mayer, J.D., y Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? En P. Salovey, D. Sluyter (ed.), Emotional Development and Emotional Intelligence: Educational Implications (pp. 3-31). New York: Basic Books.

Mitchell, J. M., Fields, H. L., D’Esposito, M., y Boettiger, C. A. (2005). Impulsive responding in alcoholics. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 29, 2158–2169. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.alc.0000191755.63639.4a

Mitchell, S. H. (1999). Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146, 455–464. https://doi.org/10.1007/pl00005491

Monterosso, J. R., Ainslie, G., Xu, J., Cordova, X., Domier, C. P., y London, E. D. (2007). Frontoparietal cortical activity of methamphetamine-dependent and comparison subjects performing a delay discounting task. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 383–393. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20281

Nathanson, L, Rivers, S. E., Flynn, L. M., y Brackett, M. A. (2016). Creating emotionally intelligent schools with ruler. Emotion Review, 8, 305–310. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073916650495

Petry, N. M. (2001). Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls. Psychopharmacology, 154, 243–250. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002130000638

Rachlin, H. (1995). Self-control: Beyond commitment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 109–121. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00037602

Reimers, S., Maylor, E. A., Stewart, N., y Chater, N. (2009). Associations between a one-shot delay discounting measure and age, income, education and real-world impulsive behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 973–978. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.07.026

Rivers, S. E. y Brackett, M. A. (2011). Achieving standards in the english language arts (and more) using the RULER Approach to social and emotional learning. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 27, 75–100. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573569.2011.532715

Rung, J. M., y Madden, G. J. (2018). Experimental reductions of delay discounting and impulsive choice: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(9), 1349-1381. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000462

Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S. L., Turvey, C., y Palfai, T. P. (1995). Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: exploring emotional intelligence using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. En J. W. Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, Disclosure, and Health (pp. 125-154). Washington: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10182-006

Schroeder, S. A. (2007). Shattuck Lecture. We can do better—Improving the health of the American people. The New England Journal of Medicine, 357, 1221–1228. https://doi.org/10.1056/nejmsa073350

Steinberg, L. (2010). A dual systems model of adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Psychobiology, 52 (3), 216–224. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20445

Thayer, J. F., Rossy, L. A., Ruiz-Padial, E., y Johnsen, B. H. (2003). Gender Differences in the Relationship between Emotional Regulation and Depressive Symptoms. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 349-364. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023922618287

Volkow, N. D., y Baler, R. D. (2015). NOW vs LATER brain circuits: Implications for obesity and addiction. Trends in Neurosciences, 38, 345–352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2015.04.002

Vuchinich, R. E., y Simpson, C. A. (1998). Hyperbolic temporal discounting in social drinkers and problem drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 6, 292–305. https://doi.org/10.1037//1064-1297.6.3.292

Published

2018-12-31

How to Cite

Sánchez López, M. T. ., Megías Robles, A. ., Gómez Leal, R. ., Gutiérrez Cobo, M. J. ., & Fernández Berrocal, P. . (2018). The Relationship Between Perceived Emotional Intelligence and Risk Behaviour in the Setting of Health. Escritos De Psicología - Psychological Writings, 11(3), 115–123. Retrieved from https://revistas.uma.es/index.php/espsi/article/view/9282

Issue

Section

Research Reports