Retroactive interference between cues trained apart: empirical evidence and theory

Authors

  • Cristina Orgaz Universidad de Deusto Spain
  • David Luque Universidad de Málaga Spain
  • Miguel A. Vadillo Universidad de Deusto Spain
  • Francisco J. López Universidad de Málaga Spain
  • Helena Matute Universidad de Deusto Spain

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.24310/espsiescpsi.v2i1.13362

Keywords:

Retroactive interference, associative learning, causal learning

Abstract

Retroactive interference between cues trained apart was long ago studied in the psychology of memory, within the paired associate tradition. Current theories of learning, however, predict that interference between cues should not occur if they are trained elementally. Here we review the available evidence on retroactive interference between cues trained apart and show that this effect is very similar to other, classical effects, in the area of learning, such as interference between outcomes and competition between cues. We suggest that a stronger connection between these research areas is important, as common mechanisms are quite possibly responsible for all these effects. Finally, we discuss whether associative or the causal inference mechanisms currently studied in the area of learning could provide a satisfactory explanation for these effects.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Metrics

Metrics Loading ...

References

Amundson, J. C., y Miller, R. R. (2007). Similarity in spatial origin of information facilitates cue competition and interference. Learning & Motivation, 38, 155-171.

Barnet, R. C., Grahame, N. J., y Miller, R. R. (1993) Temporal encoding as a determinant of blocking Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 19, 327-341.

Blaisdell, A. P., Denniston, J. C., y Miller, R. R. (1998) Temporal encoding as a determinant of overshadowing Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 24, 72-83.

Bouton, M. E. (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 80-99.

Bouton, M. E. (1997). Signals for whether versus when an event will occur. En M. E. Bouton y M. S Fanselow (Eds.), Learning, motivation, and cognition: The functional behaviorism of Robert C. Bolles (pp. 385-409). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Brooks, D. C., y Bouton, M. E. (1993). A retrieval cue for extinction attenuates spontaneous recovery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 19, 77-89.

Brooks, D. C., Palmatier, M. I., Garcia, E. O., y Johnson, J. L. (1999). An extinction cue reduces spontaneous recovery of a conditioned taste aversion. Animal Learning & Behavior, 27, 77-88.

Cobos, P. L., López, F. J. y Luque, D. (2007). Interference between cues of the same outcome depends on the causal interpretation of the events. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 369-386 .

Cole, R. P., Gunther, L. M., y Miller, R. R. (1997) Spontaneous recovery from the effect of relative stimulus validity. Learning and Motivation, 28, 1-19.

Denniston, J. C., Savastano, H. I., y Miller, R. R (2001). The extended comparator hypothesis: Learning by contiguity, responding by relative strength. En R. R. Mowrer y S. B. Klein (Eds.), Handbook of contemporary learning theories (pp. 65- 117). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Dickinson, A. y Burke, J. (1996). Within-compound associations mediate the retrospective revaluation of causality judgments. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49B, 60-80.

Escobar, M., Arcediano, F., y Miller, R. R. (2001) Conditions favoring retroactive interference between antecedents events (cue competition) and between subsequent events (outcome competition). Psychonomic, Bulletin & Review, 8, 691-697.

Escobar, M., y Miller, R. R. (2003). Timing in retroactive interference. Learning & Behavior, 31, 257-272.

Escobar, M., Pineño, O., y Matute, H. (2002). A comparison between elemental and compound training of cues in retrospective revaluation. Animal Learning & Behavior, 30, 228-238.

Griffiths, T. L. y Tenenbaum, J. B. (2005). Structure and strength in causal induction. Cognitive Psychology, 51, 334-384 .

Kamin, L. J. (1968). “Attention-like” processes in classical conditioning. En M.R. Jones, Miami symposium on the predictor of behavior: Aversive stimulation (pp. 9-31). Miami, FL: University of Miami Press.

Kraemer, P. J., Lariviere, N. A., y Spear, N. E. (1988) Expression of a taste aversion conditioned with an odor-taste compound: Overshadowing is relatively weak in weanlings and decreases over a retention interval in adults. Animal Learning & Behavior, 16,164-168.

Lagnado, D., Waldmann, M. R., Hagmayer, Y. y Sloman, S. (2007). Beyond covariation: Cues to causal structure. En A. Gopnik y L. Schulz (Eds.), Causal learning: Psychology, philosophy, and computation (pp 154-172). Nueva York: Oxford University Press.

López, F. J., Cobos, P. L. y Caño, A. (2005). Associative and causal reasoning accounts of causal induction: Symmetries and asymmetries in predictive and diagnostic inferences. Memory and Cognition, 33, 1388-1398.

López, F. J. y Shanks, D. R. (2008). Models of animal learning and their relations to human learning. En R Sun (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Computational Psychology (pp. 589-611). Cambridge, Ma.: Cambridge University Press.

Luque, D., Morís, J., Cobos, P. L. y López, F. J. (en prensa). Interference between cues of the same outcome in a non-causally framed scenario. Behavioral Processes.

Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement Psychological Review, 82, 276-298.

Matute, H., y Pineño, O. (1998). Cue competition in the absence of compound training: Its relation to paradigms of interference between outcomes. En D L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 38 (pp. 45-81). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Miller, J. S., Jagielo, J. A., y Spear, N. E. (1993). The influence of retention interval on the US preexposure effect: Changes in contextual blocking over time. Learning and Motivation, 24, 376-394.

Miller, R. R., y Escobar, M. (2002). Associative interference between cues and between outcomes presented together and presented apart: An integration Behavioural Processes, 57, 163-185.

Miller, R. R., y Matzel, L. D. (1988). The comparator hypothesis: A response rule for the expression of associations. En G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 22, pp. 51-92). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex Translated and Edited by G. V. Anrep. London: Oxford University Press.

Pearce, J. M., y Hall, G. (1980). A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 87, 532-552.

Pineño, O., y Matute, H. (2000). Interference in human predictive learning when associations share a common element. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 13, 16-33.

Pineño, O., Ortega, N., y Matute, H. (2000). The relative activation of associations modulates interference between elementally trained cues. Learning & Motivation, 31, 128-152.

Pineño, O., Urushihara, K., y Miller, R. R. (2005) Spontaneous recovery from forward and backward blocking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 31, 172-183.

Rescorla, R. A. (1968). Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66, 1-5.

Rescorla, R. A. (2006). Deepened extinction from compound stimulus presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 135-144.

Rescorla, R. A., y Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. En A H. Black y W. F. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 64-99). New York: Appelton-Century-Crofts.

Rosas, J.M., & Bouton, M.E. (1997). Additivity of the effects of retention interval and context change on latent inhibition: Toward resolution of the context forgetting paradox. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 23, 283-294.

Shanks, D. R. (1985). Forward and backward blocking in human contingency judgment. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37B, 1-21.

Slamecka, N. J., y Ceraso, J. (1960). Retroactive and proactive inhibition of verbal learning. Psychological Bulletin, 57, 449-475.

Stout, S. C., Chang, R., y Miller, R. R. (2003). Trial spacing is a determinant of cue interaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 29, 23-38.

Stout, S. C., y Miller, R. R. (2007). Sometimes-competing retrieval (SOCR): A formalization of the comparator hypothesis. Psychological Review, 114, 759-783.

Urcelay, G. P., y Miller, R. R. (2006). Counteraction between overshadowing and degraded contingency treatments: Support for the extended comparator hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 21-32.

Vadillo, M. A., Castro, L., Matute, H., y Wasserman, E. A. (2008). Backward blocking: The role of within- compound associations and interference between cues trained apart. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 185-193.

Vadillo, M. A., Orgaz, C., y Matute, H. (2008). Overshadowed cues have reduced ability to retroactively interfere with other cues. Learning & Motivation, 39, 313-322.

Van Hamme, L. J., y Wasserman, E. A. (1994). Cue competition in causality judgments: The role of nonpresentation of compound stimulus elements. Learning and Motivation, 25, 127-151.

Wagner, A. R. (1981). SOP: A model of automatic memory processing in animal behavior. En N. E Spear y R. R. Miller (Eds.): Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms (pp. 5-47). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Wagner, A. R., Logan, F. A., Haberlandt, K., y Price, T (1968). Stimulus selection in animal discrimination learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 171- 180.

Waldmann, M. R. (2000). Competition among causes but not effects in predictive and diagnostic learning Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 53-76.

Waldmann, M. R. (2001). Predictive versus diagnostic causal learning: Evidence from an overshadowing paradigm Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 600-608.

Waldmann, M. R. y Holyoak, K. J. (1992). Predictive and diagnostic learning within causal models: Asymmetries in cue competition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 222-236.

Wheeler, D. S., y Miller, R. R. (2007). Interaction between retroactive-interference and context-mediated treatments that impair Pavlovian conditioned responding. Learning & Behavior, 35, 27-35.

Published

2008-12-31

How to Cite

Orgaz, C., Luque, D., Vadillo, M. A., López, F. J., & Matute, H. (2008). Retroactive interference between cues trained apart: empirical evidence and theory. Escritos De Psicología - Psychological Writings, 2(1), 85–96. https://doi.org/10.24310/espsiescpsi.v2i1.13362

Issue

Section

Reports