Semantic richness effect at different levels of lexical-semantic processing

Authors

  • Mauro Fragapane Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Nacional de Mar del Plata Argentina
  • Leticia Vivas Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Nacional de Mar del Plata Argentina

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.24310/espsiescpsi.v10i2.13180

Keywords:

Semantic Richness, Number of Features, Lexical Decision, Semantic Categorization

Abstract

Semantic richness is a multidimensional construct that refers to the extent of variability of information associated with the meaning of a word. The Number of Features (NoF) is a dimension of semantic richness that has been shown to have a major influence on lexical and semantic processing. Several studies have shown that concepts with a higher NoF allow faster lexical processing than those with a lower NoF. The current study is the first to use a NoF measure based on norms obtained from a sample of Spanish-speaking participants. The aim was to study the effect of this variable in visual word recognition. The sample included 90 young native Spanish-speaking adults. Three tasks were administered that require access to different lexico-semantic levels: lexical decision, concreteness semantic categorization (concrete/abstract), and domain semantic categorization (living/non-living). A semantic richness effect was found in lexical decision and domain semantic categorization tasks, with greater effect in the latter task. Results are interpreted within the framework of the General Domain Interactive Activation model.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Metrics

Metrics Loading ...

References

Algarabel, S. (1996). Índices de interés psicolingüístico de 1.917 palabras castellanas. Cognitiva, 8, 43-88. https://doi.org/10.1174/021435596321235298

Alija, M. y Cuetos, F. (2006). Efectos de las variables léxico – semánticas en el reconocimiento visual. Psicothema, 18, 485-491.

Aron, A. y Aron, E.N. (2001). Estadística para psicología. Segunda edición. Buenos Aires: Pearson Education.

Balota, D. A. (1990). The role of meaning in word recognition. En D. A. Balota, G. B. Flores d’Arcais, y K. Rayner (Eds.), Comprehension processes in reading (pp. 9-32). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Brysbaert, M., Van Wijnendaele, I., y De Deyne, S. (2000). Age-of-acquisition of words is a significant variable in semantic tasks. Acta Psychologica, 104, 215-226. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(00)00021-4

Caramazza, A. y Shelton, J.R. (1998). Domain specific knowledge systems in the brain: The animate–inanimate distinction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 1-34. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892998563752

Chen, Q. y Mirman, D. (2012). Competition and cooperation among similar representations: toward a unified account of facilitative and inhibitory effects of lexical neighbors. Psychological Review, 119 (2), 417–430. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027175

Cycowicz, Y. M., Friedman, D., Rothstein, M., y Snodgrass, J. G. (1997). Picture naming by young children: Norms for name agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 65, 171-237. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1996.2356

Declaración de Helsinki de la AMM. Principios éticos para las investigaciones médicas en seres humanos. 64th Asamblea Geneal, Fortaleza, Brasil, octubre 2013.

Ferreira, R. A., Göbel, S. M., Hymers, M., y Ellis, A. W. (2015). The neural correlates of semantic richness: Evidence from an fMRI study of word learning. Brain and Language, 143, 69-80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2015.02.005

Grondin, R., Lupker S., y McRae K. (2009). Shared features dominate semantic richness effects for concrete concepts. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 1-19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.09.001

Hino, Y., Pexman, P.M., y Lupker, S.J. (2006). Ambiguity and relatedness effects in semantic tasks: Are they due to semantic coding? Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 247-273. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.04.001

Kremer, G. y Baroni, M. (2011). A set of semantic norms for German and Italian. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 97-109. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-010-0028-x

Manoiloff, L., Artstein, M., Canavoso, M., Fernández, L., y Segui, J. (2010). Expanded norms for 400 experimental pictures in an Argentinean Spanishspeaking population. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 452-460. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.2.452

Martínez-Cuitiño, M.; Barreyro, J.P.; Wilson, M. y Jaichenco, V. (2015). Nuevas normas semánticas y de tiempos de latencia para un set de 400 dibujos en español. Interdisciplinaria, 32, 289-305.

McRae, K., Cree, G. S., Seidenberg, M. S., y McNorgan, C. (2005). Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and nonliving things. Behavior Research Methods, 37(4), 547-559. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192726

Moss, H.E., Tyler, L.K., y Taylor, K.I. (2007). Conceptual structure. En: G. Gaskell (Ed.), Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 217-234). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198568971.013.0013

Pexman, P., Lupker S. y Hino, (2002). The impact of feedback semantics in visual Word recognition: Number-of-features effects in lexical decision and naming tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 542-549. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196311

Pexman, P., Holyk G., y Monfiels M. (2003). Number-of-features effects and semantic processing.

Pexman, P., Hargreaves I., Siakaluk P., Bodner G., y Pope J. (2008). There are many ways to be rich: Effects of three measures of semantic richness on visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 161-167. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.1.161

Sajin, S. y Connine C. (2014). Semantic richness: The role of semantic features in processing spoken words. Journal of Memory and Language, 70, 13-35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2013.09.006

Vivas, J., Vivas, L., Comesaña, A., Coni. A.G., Vorano, A. (2017). Spanish Semantic Feature Production Norms for 400 concrete concepts. Behavior Research Methods, 49, 1095-1106. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0777-2

Yap, M., Tan, S., Pexman P. y Hargreaves, I. (2011). Is more always better? Effects of semantic richness on lexical decision, speeded pronunciation, and semantic classification. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 742-750. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0092-y

Yap, M. y Pexman, P. (2015). Semantic richness effect in lexical decision: the role of feedback. Memory & Cognition, 43, 1148-1167. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-015-0536-0

Yap, M., Pexman P., Wellsby M., Hargreaves I. y Huff M. (2012). An abundance of richess: crosstask comparisons of semantic richness effects in visual word recognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 1-10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00072

Published

2017-09-01

How to Cite

Fragapane, M., & Vivas, L. (2017). Semantic richness effect at different levels of lexical-semantic processing. Escritos De Psicología - Psychological Writings, 10(2), 88–102. https://doi.org/10.24310/espsiescpsi.v10i2.13180

Issue

Section

Research Reports