Interference Effect in neutral and emotional contexts: a theoretical framework
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.24310/espsiescpsi.v13i1.10079Keywords:
interference, inhibition, emotion, emotional stimuliAbstract
Interference is the experimental effect caused by distracting information. It manifests as decreased performance in conditions with distracting information compared to performance in conditions without distracting information. Although many studies have investigated this effect, most of them have analysed it in neutral settings (i.e. settings without emotional salience). We review the interference effect, its temporal and formal dimensions, and settings in which it occurs (i.e. in neutral as well as emotionally charged settings), and different aspects of emotional settings that modulate the interference effect. Finally, we include different concepts that support the interference effect as the main explanatory mechanism of inhibitory control processes.
Downloads
Metrics
References
Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1-24. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1
Bradley, M. M., Hamby, S., Löw, A., & Lang, P. J. (2007). Brain potentials in perception: picture complexity and emotional arousal. Psychophysiology, 44(3), 364-373. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00520.x
Canet-Juric, L., Introzzi, I., & Zamora, E. (2016). Inhibición o inhibiciones: No siempre inhibimos lo mismo. En I. Introzzi & L. Canet Juric (Comps.), ¿Quién dirige la batuta? Funciones Ejecutivas: herramientas para la regulación de la mente, la emoción y la acción (pp. 40-59), Mar del Plata: EUDEM.
Carretié, L., Albert, J., López-Martín, S., & Tapia, M. (2009). Negative brain: an integrative review on the neural processes activated by unpleasant stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 71, 57-63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.07.006
Carretié, L., López-Martín, S., & Albert, J. (2010). Papel de la corteza prefrontal ventromedial en la respuesta a eventos emocionalmente negativos. Revista de Neurología, 50, 245-252. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5004.2009292
Casey, B. J. (2000). Disruption of inhibitory control in developmental disorders: A mechanistic model of implicated frontostriatal circuitry. En R. S. Siegler & J. L. McClelland (Eds.), Mechanisms of cognitive development: The Carnegie Symposium on cognition (Vol. 28, pp. 327-349). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cohen, A. O., Dellarco, D. V., Breiner, K., Helion, C., Heller, A. S., Rahdar, A., ... & Casey, B. J. (2016). The impact of emotional states on cognitive control circuitry and function. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28, 446-459. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00906
Cohen, N., & Henik, A. (2012). Do irrelevant emotional stimuli impair or improve executive control?.Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 1-4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2012.00033
Compas, B. E., Jaser, S. S., Bettis, A. H., Watson, K. H., Gruhn, M. A., Dunbar, J. P., ... & Thigpen, J. C. (2017). Coping, emotion regulation, and psychopathology in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 939-991. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000110
Compton, R. J., Banich, M. T., Mohanty, A., Milham, M., Herrington, J., Miller, G., …& Heller, W. (2003). Paying attention to emotion: an fMRI investigation of cognitive and emotional Stroop tasks. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 81-96. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.3.2.81
Dempster, F. N. (1993). Resistance to interference: Developmental changes in a basic processing dimension. En M. L. Howe & R. Pasnak (Eds.), Emerging themes in cognitive development (Vol. 1, pp. 3-27). New York: Springer–Verlag.
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135-168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
Diamond, A. (2016). Why improving and assessing executive functions early in life is critical. En J. A. Griffin, P. McCardle & L. S. Freund (Eds.), Executive function in preschool-age children: Integrating measurement, neurodevelopment, and translational research (pp. 11-43). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/110.13140/RG.2.1.2644.6483
Edwards, W. H. (2010). Motor Learning and Control: From Theory to Practice. Belmont, California: Cengage Learning.
Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.). (1994). The nature of emotion. New York: Oxford University Press.
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a non-search task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143-149. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203267
Eugène, F., Lévesque, J., Mensour, B., Leroux, J.-M., Beaudoin, G., Bourgouin, P., & Beauregard, M. (2003). The impact of individual differences on the neural circuitry underlying emotion. NeuroImage, 19, 354-364. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00121-6
Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2008). Failures to ignore entirely irrelevant distractors: The role of load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 73-83. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.14.1.73
Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2016). Establishing the attention-distractibility trait. Psychological Science, 27, 203-212 https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615617761
Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681-700. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.130.4.681
Glaser, W. R. (1992). Picture naming. Cognition, 42, 61-105. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(92)90040-O
Glaser, W. R., & Glaser, M. O. (1989). Context effects in stroop-like word and picture processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 13. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.118.1.13
Gotlib, I. H., & McCann, C. D. (1984). Construct accessibility and depression: An examination of cognitive and affective factors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 427-439. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.47.2.427
Harnishfeger, K. K. (1995). The development of cognitive inhibition: Theories, definitions, and research evidence. En F. Dempster & C. Brainerd (Eds.), Interference and inhibition in cognition (pp. 175-204). San Diego: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012208930-5/50007-6
Hasson, U., Malach, R., & Heeger, D. J. (2010). Reliability of cortical activity during natural stimulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 40-48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.10.011
Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 174-180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006
Houwer, J. D., & Hermans, D. (1994). Differences in the affective processing of words and pictures. Cognition & Emotion, 8, 1-20. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939408408925
Iordan, A. D., Dolcos, S., & Dolcos, F. (2013). Neural signatures of the response to emotional distraction: a review of evidence from brain imaging investigations. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 200. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00200
Jacoby, L. L., Debner, J. A., & Hay, J. F. (2001). Proactive interference, accessibility bias, and process dissociations: Valid subject reports of memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 686-700. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.27.3.686
Jones, A. D., Cho, R. Y., Nystrom, L. E., Cohen, J. D., & Braver, T. S. (2002). A computational model of anterior cingulate function in speeded response tasks: Effects of frequency, sequence, and conflict. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 300-317. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.2.4.300
Klein, G. S. (1964). Semantic power measured through the interference of words with color naming. American Journal of Psychology, 77, 576-588. https://doi.org/10.2307/1420768
Kron, A., Schul, Y., Cohen, A., & Hassin, R. R. (2010). Feelings don't come easy: Studies on the effortful nature of feelings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(3), 520-534. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020008
Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261–273. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03352.x
Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 451-468. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.21.3.451
Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused? Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 75-82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.004
Lavie, N., & Tsal, Y. (1994). Perceptual load as a major determinant of the locus of selection in visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 183-197. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03213897
Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339-354. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339
LeDoux, J. E. (2015). Anxious: Using the brain to understand and treat fear and anxiety. New York: Penguin.
LeDoux, J., & Phelps, E. (2008). Emotional networks in the brain. En L. Feldman Barret, M. Lewis, & J. Haviland-Jones (eds.) Handbook of Emotion, (3rd ed., pp. 159-179). New York: Guilford.
Lees, A., Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Health anxiety, anxiety sensitivity, and attentional biases for pictorial and linguistic health - threat cues. Cognition & Emotion, 19, 453-462. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930441000184
Lichtenstein-Vidne, L., Henik, A., Safadi, Z., (2012). Task relevance modulates processing of distracting emotional stimuli. Cognition & Emotion, 26, 42–52. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2011.567055
MacLeod, C. M. (2007). The concept of inhibition in cognition. En D. S. Gorfein & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.), Inhibition in cognition (pp. 3-23). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/11587-001
MacLeod, C. M., Dodd, M. D., Sheard, E. D., Wilson, D. E., & Bibi, U. (2003). In opposition to inhibition. En B. H. Ross (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation - Advances in research and theory (Vol. 43, pp. 163-214). London: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(03)01014-4
Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annual Review Clinical Psychology, 1, 167-195. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143916
Mauss, I. B., & Robinson, M. D. (2009). Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition & Emotion, 23, 209–237. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802204677
McGeoch, J. A., & McDonald, W. T. (1931). Meaningful relation and retroactive inhibition. The American Journal of Psychology, 43, 579-588. https://doi.org/10.2307/1415159
McKenna, F. P., & Sharma, D. (2004). Reversing the emotional Stroop effect reveals that it is not what it seems: the role of fast and slow components. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30, 382-392. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.382
McKenna, F. P., & Sharma, D. (2004). Reversing the emotional Stroop effect reveals that it is not what it seems: the role of fast and slow components. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30, 382-392. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.382
Mishra, J., Anguera, J. A., Ziegler, D. A., & Gazzaley, A. (2013). A cognitive framework for understanding and improving interference resolution in the brain. Progress in Brain Research, 207, 351-377. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-63327-9.00013-8
Okon?Singer, H., Kofman, O., Tzelgov, J., & Henik, A. (2011). Using international emotional picture sets in countries suffering from violence. Journal of traumatic stress, 24, 239-242. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20600
Okon-Singer, H., Lichtenstein-Vidne, L., & Cohen, N. (2013). Dynamic modulation of emotional processing. Biological Psychology, 92, 480-491. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.05.010
Okon-Singer, H., Tzelgov, J., & Henik, A., (2007). Distinguishing between automaticity and attention in the processing of emotionally significant stimuli. Emotion, 7, 147–157. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.147
Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380-391. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.380
Price, C. J. (2010). The anatomy of language: a review of 100 fMRI studies published in 2009. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1191, 62-88. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05444.x
Rebetez, M. M. L., Rochat, L., Billieux, J., Gay, P., & Van der Linden, M. (2015). Do emotional stimuli interfere with two distinct components of inhibition? Cognition & Emotion, 29, 559-567. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2014.922054
Réveillon, M., Lazeyras, F., Van Calster, L., Cojan, Y., Sander, D., Hüppi, P. S., & Barisnikov, K. (2018, en prensa). Neural functional correlates of the impact of socio-emotional stimuli on performances on a flanker task in children aged 9–11 years. Neuropsychologia. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.004
Schacht, A., Sommer, W., (2009). Emotions in word and face processing: early and late cortical responses. Brain and Cognition, 69, 538-550. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2008.11.005
Schimmack, U. (2005). Attentional interference effects of emotional pictures: threat, negativity, or arousal? Emotion, 5, 55-66. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.55
Simon, J. R., & Berbaum, K. (1990). Effect of conflicting cues on information processing: the ‘Stroop effect’vs. the ‘Simon effect’. Acta Psychologica, 73, 159-170. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(90)90077-S
Song, S., Zilverstand, A., Song, H., Uquillas, F. D. O., Wang, Y., Xie, C., ... & Zou, Z. (2017). The influence of emotional interference on cognitive control: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies using the emotional Stroop task. Scientific Reports, 7, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-02266-2
Underwood, B. J. (1957). Interference and forgetting. Psychological Review, 64, 49-60. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0044616
van der Molen, M. W. (2000). Developmental changes in inhibitory processing: Evidence from psychophysiological measures. Biological Psychology, 54, 207-239. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0511(00)00057-0
Vuilleumier, P., Sagiv, N., Hazeltine, E., Poldrack, R. A., Swick, D., Rafal, R. D., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2001). Neural fate of seen and unseen faces in visuospatial neglect: a combined event-related functional MRI and event-related potential study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 3495-3500. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.051436898
Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3-24. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3
Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 3-47. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930903205698
Zamora, E. V, Introzzi, I., del Valle, M., Vernucci, S., Richard’s, M. (2020). Perceptual inhibition of emotional interference. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 9(3),215-229. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2019.1567340
Ziegler, D. A., Janowich, J. R., & Gazzaley, A. (2018). Differential Impact of Interference on Internally-and Externally - Directed Attention. Scientific Reports, 8, 2498. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-20498-8
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
All contents published in Escritos de Psicología are protected under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-SA 4.0) license. All about this license is available in the following link: <http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0>
Users can copy, use, redistribute, share and exhibit publicly as long as:
- The original source and authorship of the material are cited (Journal, Publisher and URL of the work).
- It is not used for comercial purposes.
- The existence of the license and its especifications are mentioned.
There are two sets of authors’ rights: moral and property rights. Moral rights are perpetual prerogatives, unrenounceable, not-transferable, unalienable, imprescriptible and inembargable. According to authors’ rights legislation, Escritos de Psicología recognizes and respects authors moral rights, as well as the ownership of property rights. The property rights are referred to the benefits that are gained by the use or the dissemination of works. Escritos de Psicología is published in an open access form and it is exclusively licenced by any means for doing or authorising distribution, dissemination, reproduction, , adaptation, translation or arrangement of works.
Authors are responsable for obtaining the necessary permission to use copyrighted images.