Marco teórico del efecto de interferencia en contextos neutrales y emocionales

Autores/as

  • Eliana V. Zamora Instituto de Psicología Básica, Aplicada y Tecnología (IPSIBAT), Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET) y Universidad Nacional de Mar del Plata (UNMDP), Argentina Argentina https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6278-6665
  • Isabel Introzzi Instituto de Psicología Básica, Aplicada y Tecnología (IPSIBAT), Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET) y Universidad Nacional de Mar del Plata (UNMDP), Argentina Argentina
  • Macarena del Valle Instituto de Psicología Básica, Aplicada y Tecnología (IPSIBAT), Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET) y Universidad Nacional de Mar del Plata (UNMDP), Argentina Argentina https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3549-7224
  • María Richard's Instituto de Psicología Básica, Aplicada y Tecnología (IPSIBAT), Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET) y Universidad Nacional de Mar del Plata (UNMDP), Argentina Argentina https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7394-5967

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.24310/espsiescpsi.v13i1.10079

Palabras clave:

interferencia, inhibición, emoción, estímulos emocionales

Resumen

La interferencia es el efecto experimental causado por información distractora que se manifiesta en una disminución del desempeño respecto a condiciones que implican ausencia de información distractora. Si bien existe un notorio cuerpo de literatura respecto a este efecto, la gran mayoría de los trabajos existentes lo analizan en contextos neutrales, es decir, sin saliencia emocional. Por este motivo, en este trabajo se revisará el efecto de interferencia, sus dimensiones temporales y formales, como así también los contextos en los que interviene, es decir no sólo los neutrales sino también aquellos con saliencia emocional. Con respecto a estos últimos, se revisan los diferentes aspectos de los contextos emocionales que modulan el efecto de interferencia. Finalmente, se incorporan diferentes nociones que han inclinado la balanza a favor del efecto de interferencia como el principal mecanismo explicativo de los procesos de control inhibitorio.

Descargas

Los datos de descargas todavía no están disponibles.

Métricas

Cargando métricas ...

Citas

Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1-24. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1

Bradley, M. M., Hamby, S., Löw, A., & Lang, P. J. (2007). Brain potentials in perception: picture complexity and emotional arousal. Psychophysiology, 44(3), 364-373. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00520.x

Canet-Juric, L., Introzzi, I., & Zamora, E. (2016). Inhibición o inhibiciones: No siempre inhibimos lo mismo. En I. Introzzi & L. Canet Juric (Comps.), ¿Quién dirige la batuta? Funciones Ejecutivas: herramientas para la regulación de la mente, la emoción y la acción (pp. 40-59), Mar del Plata: EUDEM.

Carretié, L., Albert, J., López-Martín, S., & Tapia, M. (2009). Negative brain: an integrative review on the neural processes activated by unpleasant stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 71, 57-63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.07.006

Carretié, L., López-Martín, S., & Albert, J. (2010). Papel de la corteza prefrontal ventromedial en la respuesta a eventos emocionalmente negativos. Revista de Neurología, 50, 245-252. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5004.2009292

Casey, B. J. (2000). Disruption of inhibitory control in developmental disorders: A mechanistic model of implicated frontostriatal circuitry. En R. S. Siegler & J. L. McClelland (Eds.), Mechanisms of cognitive development: The Carnegie Symposium on cognition (Vol. 28, pp. 327-349). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Cohen, A. O., Dellarco, D. V., Breiner, K., Helion, C., Heller, A. S., Rahdar, A., ... & Casey, B. J. (2016). The impact of emotional states on cognitive control circuitry and function. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28, 446-459. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00906

Cohen, N., & Henik, A. (2012). Do irrelevant emotional stimuli impair or improve executive control?.Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 1-4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2012.00033

Compas, B. E., Jaser, S. S., Bettis, A. H., Watson, K. H., Gruhn, M. A., Dunbar, J. P., ... & Thigpen, J. C. (2017). Coping, emotion regulation, and psychopathology in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 939-991. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000110

Compton, R. J., Banich, M. T., Mohanty, A., Milham, M., Herrington, J., Miller, G., …& Heller, W. (2003). Paying attention to emotion: an fMRI investigation of cognitive and emotional Stroop tasks. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 81-96. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.3.2.81

Dempster, F. N. (1993). Resistance to interference: Developmental changes in a basic processing dimension. En M. L. Howe & R. Pasnak (Eds.), Emerging themes in cognitive development (Vol. 1, pp. 3-27). New York: Springer–Verlag.

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135-168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

Diamond, A. (2016). Why improving and assessing executive functions early in life is critical. En J. A. Griffin, P. McCardle & L. S. Freund (Eds.), Executive function in preschool-age children: Integrating measurement, neurodevelopment, and translational research (pp. 11-43). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/110.13140/RG.2.1.2644.6483

Edwards, W. H. (2010). Motor Learning and Control: From Theory to Practice. Belmont, California: Cengage Learning.

Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.). (1994). The nature of emotion. New York: Oxford University Press.

Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a non-search task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143-149. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203267

Eugène, F., Lévesque, J., Mensour, B., Leroux, J.-M., Beaudoin, G., Bourgouin, P., & Beauregard, M. (2003). The impact of individual differences on the neural circuitry underlying emotion. NeuroImage, 19, 354-364. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00121-6

Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2008). Failures to ignore entirely irrelevant distractors: The role of load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 73-83. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.14.1.73

Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2016). Establishing the attention-distractibility trait. Psychological Science, 27, 203-212 https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615617761

Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681-700. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.130.4.681

Glaser, W. R. (1992). Picture naming. Cognition, 42, 61-105. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(92)90040-O

Glaser, W. R., & Glaser, M. O. (1989). Context effects in stroop-like word and picture processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 13. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.118.1.13

Gotlib, I. H., & McCann, C. D. (1984). Construct accessibility and depression: An examination of cognitive and affective factors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 427-439. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.47.2.427

Harnishfeger, K. K. (1995). The development of cognitive inhibition: Theories, definitions, and research evidence. En F. Dempster & C. Brainerd (Eds.), Interference and inhibition in cognition (pp. 175-204). San Diego: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012208930-5/50007-6

Hasson, U., Malach, R., & Heeger, D. J. (2010). Reliability of cortical activity during natural stimulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 40-48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.10.011

Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 174-180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006

Houwer, J. D., & Hermans, D. (1994). Differences in the affective processing of words and pictures. Cognition & Emotion, 8, 1-20. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939408408925

Iordan, A. D., Dolcos, S., & Dolcos, F. (2013). Neural signatures of the response to emotional distraction: a review of evidence from brain imaging investigations. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 200. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00200

Jacoby, L. L., Debner, J. A., & Hay, J. F. (2001). Proactive interference, accessibility bias, and process dissociations: Valid subject reports of memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 686-700. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.27.3.686

Jones, A. D., Cho, R. Y., Nystrom, L. E., Cohen, J. D., & Braver, T. S. (2002). A computational model of anterior cingulate function in speeded response tasks: Effects of frequency, sequence, and conflict. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 300-317. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.2.4.300

Klein, G. S. (1964). Semantic power measured through the interference of words with color naming. American Journal of Psychology, 77, 576-588. https://doi.org/10.2307/1420768

Kron, A., Schul, Y., Cohen, A., & Hassin, R. R. (2010). Feelings don't come easy: Studies on the effortful nature of feelings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(3), 520-534. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020008

Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261–273. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03352.x

Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 451-468. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.21.3.451

Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused? Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 75-82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.004

Lavie, N., & Tsal, Y. (1994). Perceptual load as a major determinant of the locus of selection in visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 183-197. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03213897

Lavie, N., Hirst, A., De Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339-354. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339

LeDoux, J. E. (2015). Anxious: Using the brain to understand and treat fear and anxiety. New York: Penguin.

LeDoux, J., & Phelps, E. (2008). Emotional networks in the brain. En L. Feldman Barret, M. Lewis, & J. Haviland-Jones (eds.) Handbook of Emotion, (3rd ed., pp. 159-179). New York: Guilford.

Lees, A., Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Health anxiety, anxiety sensitivity, and attentional biases for pictorial and linguistic health - threat cues. Cognition & Emotion, 19, 453-462. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930441000184

Lichtenstein-Vidne, L., Henik, A., Safadi, Z., (2012). Task relevance modulates processing of distracting emotional stimuli. Cognition & Emotion, 26, 42–52. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2011.567055

MacLeod, C. M. (2007). The concept of inhibition in cognition. En D. S. Gorfein & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.), Inhibition in cognition (pp. 3-23). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/11587-001

MacLeod, C. M., Dodd, M. D., Sheard, E. D., Wilson, D. E., & Bibi, U. (2003). In opposition to inhibition. En B. H. Ross (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation - Advances in research and theory (Vol. 43, pp. 163-214). London: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(03)01014-4

Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annual Review Clinical Psychology, 1, 167-195. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143916

Mauss, I. B., & Robinson, M. D. (2009). Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition & Emotion, 23, 209–237. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802204677

McGeoch, J. A., & McDonald, W. T. (1931). Meaningful relation and retroactive inhibition. The American Journal of Psychology, 43, 579-588. https://doi.org/10.2307/1415159

McKenna, F. P., & Sharma, D. (2004). Reversing the emotional Stroop effect reveals that it is not what it seems: the role of fast and slow components. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30, 382-392. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.382

McKenna, F. P., & Sharma, D. (2004). Reversing the emotional Stroop effect reveals that it is not what it seems: the role of fast and slow components. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30, 382-392. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.382

Mishra, J., Anguera, J. A., Ziegler, D. A., & Gazzaley, A. (2013). A cognitive framework for understanding and improving interference resolution in the brain. Progress in Brain Research, 207, 351-377. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-63327-9.00013-8

Okon?Singer, H., Kofman, O., Tzelgov, J., & Henik, A. (2011). Using international emotional picture sets in countries suffering from violence. Journal of traumatic stress, 24, 239-242. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20600

Okon-Singer, H., Lichtenstein-Vidne, L., & Cohen, N. (2013). Dynamic modulation of emotional processing. Biological Psychology, 92, 480-491. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.05.010

Okon-Singer, H., Tzelgov, J., & Henik, A., (2007). Distinguishing between automaticity and attention in the processing of emotionally significant stimuli. Emotion, 7, 147–157. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.147

Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380-391. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.380

Price, C. J. (2010). The anatomy of language: a review of 100 fMRI studies published in 2009. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1191, 62-88. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05444.x

Rebetez, M. M. L., Rochat, L., Billieux, J., Gay, P., & Van der Linden, M. (2015). Do emotional stimuli interfere with two distinct components of inhibition? Cognition & Emotion, 29, 559-567. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2014.922054

Réveillon, M., Lazeyras, F., Van Calster, L., Cojan, Y., Sander, D., Hüppi, P. S., & Barisnikov, K. (2018, en prensa). Neural functional correlates of the impact of socio-emotional stimuli on performances on a flanker task in children aged 9–11 years. Neuropsychologia. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.004

Schacht, A., Sommer, W., (2009). Emotions in word and face processing: early and late cortical responses. Brain and Cognition, 69, 538-550. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2008.11.005

Schimmack, U. (2005). Attentional interference effects of emotional pictures: threat, negativity, or arousal? Emotion, 5, 55-66. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.55

Simon, J. R., & Berbaum, K. (1990). Effect of conflicting cues on information processing: the ‘Stroop effect’vs. the ‘Simon effect’. Acta Psychologica, 73, 159-170. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(90)90077-S

Song, S., Zilverstand, A., Song, H., Uquillas, F. D. O., Wang, Y., Xie, C., ... & Zou, Z. (2017). The influence of emotional interference on cognitive control: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies using the emotional Stroop task. Scientific Reports, 7, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-02266-2

Underwood, B. J. (1957). Interference and forgetting. Psychological Review, 64, 49-60. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0044616

van der Molen, M. W. (2000). Developmental changes in inhibitory processing: Evidence from psychophysiological measures. Biological Psychology, 54, 207-239. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0511(00)00057-0

Vuilleumier, P., Sagiv, N., Hazeltine, E., Poldrack, R. A., Swick, D., Rafal, R. D., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2001). Neural fate of seen and unseen faces in visuospatial neglect: a combined event-related functional MRI and event-related potential study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 3495-3500. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.051436898

Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3-24. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3

Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 3-47. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930903205698

Zamora, E. V, Introzzi, I., del Valle, M., Vernucci, S., Richard’s, M. (2020). Perceptual inhibition of emotional interference. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 9(3),215-229. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2019.1567340

Ziegler, D. A., Janowich, J. R., & Gazzaley, A. (2018). Differential Impact of Interference on Internally-and Externally - Directed Attention. Scientific Reports, 8, 2498. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-20498-8

Descargas

Publicado

2020-07-01

Cómo citar

Zamora, E. V., Introzzi, I., del Valle , M., & Richard’s, M. (2020). Marco teórico del efecto de interferencia en contextos neutrales y emocionales . Escritos De Psicología - Psychological Writings, 13(1), 23–33. https://doi.org/10.24310/espsiescpsi.v13i1.10079

Número

Sección

Artículos