Golf putting in adolescents: random practice versus error free learning
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.24310/riccafd.12.3.2023.17895Keywords:
error reduction, variability, golf, physical education, adolescentsAbstract
Although learning golf putting has been the subject of numerous studies, in the adolescent age group there are very few references on how to design a learning process that has the appropriate challenge gradient and difficulty in physical education (PE) learning environments. The aim of this study was to investigate through an ecological approach, whether random practice (variability) facilitates learning golf putting in the initiation phase as well as error-free learning (repetition). A total of 46 students (M=13.8±.07 years) in the second year of compulsory secondary education (ESO) participated in the study. None of the participants had previous experience playing golf. The following assessments were undertaken: a) pretest, b) intervention, consisting of seven practice blocks of 10 repetitions, c) posttest and d) transfer test (two weeks after the posttest). The data indicate that both the random learning group (variability) and the error-free learning group achieved similar results in the posttest (M=1.36, SD=0.32 versus M=1.16 SD=0.30), and in the transfer test (M=2.56, SD=0.07 versus M=2.53, SD= 0.07). Learning complex technical movements such as golf putting is possible for adolescents in the initiation phase. Both random practice and error reduction provide a similar degree of learning in the short and long term.
Downloads
Metrics
References
Aiken CA, Genter AM. The effects of blocked and random practice on the learning of three variations of the golf chip shot. Int J Perform Anal Sport. 2018;18: 339–349. doi:10.1080/24748668.2018.1475199
Guadagnoll MA, Lee TD. Challenge Point: A Framework for Conceptualizing the Effects of Various Practice Conditions in Motor Learning. J Mot Behav. 2004;36: 212–224. doi:10.3200/JMBR.36.2.212-224
Conde-pipó J, Fernández-aguilera M. Propuesta metodológica de iniciación al esquí alpino escolar mediante esquís cortos Methodological proposal of initiation to alpine skiing in school children through short skis Introducción El esquí alpino es un deporte de naturaleza que se practica en la.
Duijn T van, Thomas S, Masters RSW. Chipping in on the role of conscious processing during children’s motor learning by analogy. Int J Sports Sci Coach. 2019;14: 383–392. doi:10.1177/1747954119841162
Chow JY, Renshaw I, Button C. SN RD RIB. 2013.
Kleynen M, Braun SM, Rasquin SMC, Bleijlevens MHC, Lexis MAS, Halfens J, et al. Multidisciplinary Views on Applying Explicit and Implicit Motor Learning in Practice: An International Survey. PLoS One. 2015;10: e0135522. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0135522
Richardson AK, Mitchell ACS, Hughes G. The effect of movement variability on putting proficiency during the golf putting stroke. Int J Sport Sci Coach. 2018;13: 590–597. doi:10.1177/1747954118768234
Keogh JWL, Hume PA. Evidence for biomechanics and motor learning research improving golf performance. Sports Biomechanics. 2012. doi:10.1080/14763141.2012.671354
Kamp G. van der J, Duivenvoorden J, Kok M, Hilvoorde I van. Motor Skill Learning in Groups: Some Proposals for Applying Implicit Learning and Self-Controlled Feedback. [Aprendizaje de habilidades/destrezas motoras en grupos: Propuestas para la Aplicación de Aprendizaje Implícito, autocontrolado y retroalimentado]. RICYDE Rev Int ciencias del Deport. 2015;11: 33–47. doi:10.5232/ricyde2015.03903
Maxwell JP, Masters RSW, Kerr E, Weedon E. The implicit benefit of learning without errors. Q J Exp Psychol Sect A Hum Exp Psychol. 2001;54: 1049–1068. doi:10.1080/713756014
Capio CM, Poolton JM, Sit CHP, Holmstrom M, Masters RSW. Reducing errors benefits the field-based learning of a fundamental movement skill in children. Scand J Med Sci Sport. 2013;23: 181–188. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0838.2011.01368.x
Lee MCY, Chow JY, Komar J, Tan CWK, Button C. Nonlinear pedagogy: An effective approach to cater for individual differences in learning a sports skill. PLoS One. 2014;9: e104744. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0104744
Camacho Lazarraga P, Calvo Lluch Á. Estrategia de enseñanza sin errores para el aprendizaje de las habilidades deportivas. Una revisión sistemática. Sport Sci J Sch Sport Phys Educ Psychomot. 2017;3: 621. doi:10.17979/sportis.2017.3.3.2021
Capio CM, Sit CHP, Eguia KF, Abernethy B, Masters RSW. Fundamental movement skills training to promote physical activity in children with and without disability: A pilot study. J Sport Health Sci. 2015;4: 235–243. doi:10.1016/j.jshs.2014.08.001
Buszard T, Farrow D, Verswijveren SJJM, Reid M, Williams J, Polman R, et al. Working memory capacity limits motor learning when implementing multiple instructions. Front Psychol. 2017;8: 1–12. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01350
Lopes MC, Albuquerque MR, Raab M. Effects of implicit, explicit and sequential learning in the acquisition of the basketball shooting skill in novices. J Phys Educ. 2018;29: 1–11. doi:10.4025/jphyseduc.v29i1.2964
Maxwell JP, Capio CM, Masters RSW. Interaction between motor ability and skill learning in children: Application of implicit and explicit approaches. Eur J Sport Sci. 2017;17: 407–416. doi:10.1080/17461391.2016.1268211
Brocken JEA, Kal EC, van der Kamp J. Focus of Attention in Children’s Motor Learning: Examining the Role of Age and Working Memory. J Mot Behav. 2016;48: 527–534. doi:10.1080/00222895.2016.1152224
Abswoude F Van, Nuijen NB, Kamp J Van Der. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport Individual Differences Influencing Immediate Effects of Internal and External Focus Instructions on Children ’ s Motor Performance Individual Differences Influencing Immediate Effects of Internal and External. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2018;89: 190–199. doi:10.1080/02701367.2018.1442915
Kal EC, Van Der Kamp J, Houdijk H. External attentional focus enhances movement automatization: A comprehensive test of the constrained action hypothesis. Hum Mov Sci. 2013;32: 527–539. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2013.04.001
Ruiz-Amengual A, Ruiz-Pérez LM. Práctica aleatoria y aprendizaje deportivo-motor. Rev Iberoam Psicol del Ejerc y el Deport. 2014;9: 123–142.
Porter JM, Magill RA. Systematically increasing contextual interference is beneficial for learning sport skills. J Sports Sci. 2010;28: 1277–1285. doi:10.1080/02640414.2010.502946
Chua LK, Dimapilis MK, Iwatsuki T, Abdollahipour R, Lewthwaite R, Wulf G. Practice variability promotes an external focus of attention and enhances motor skill learning. Hum Mov Sci. 2019;64: 307–319. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2019.02.015
Barreiros J, Figueiredo T, Godinho M. The contextual interference effect in applied settings. Eur Phys Educ Rev. 2007;13: 195–208. doi:10.1177/1356336X07076876
Goodwin JE, Meeuwsen H. B, b, . . . 1996; 735–743.
Guadagnoli MA, Holcomb WR, Weber TJ. The relationship between contextual interference effects and performer expertise on the learning of a putting task. J Hum Mov Stud. 1999;37: 19–36.
Harriss DJ, Macsween A, Atkinson G. Standards for Ethics in Sport and Exercise Science Research: 2018 Update. Int J Sports Med. 2017;38: 1126–1131. doi:10.1055/s-0043-124001
Review C, Communication S, Principles G. World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. J Am Coll Dent. 2014;81: 14–18. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199241323.003.0025
Barnett LM, Hardy LL, Brian AS, Robertson S. The development and validation of a golf swing and putt skill assessment for children. J Sports Sci Med. 2015;14: 147–54.
Robertson S, Burnett AF, Gupta R. Two tests of approach-iron golf skill and their ability to predict tournament performance. J Sports Sci. 2014. doi:10.1080/02640414.2014.893370
Porter JM, Landin D, Hebert EP, Baum B. The Effects of Three Levels of Contextual Interference on Performance Outcomes and Movement Patterns in Golf Skills. Int J Sport Sci Coach. 2009;2: 243–255. doi:10.1260/174795407782233100
Zhu FF, Poolton JM, Wilson MR, Maxwell JP, Masters RSW. Neural co-activation as a yardstick of implicit motor learning and the propensity for conscious control of movement. Biol Psychol. 2011;87: 66–73. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.004
D’Innocenzo G, Gonzalez CC, Williams AM, Bishop DT. Looking to learn: The effects of visual guidance on observational learning of the golf swing. PLoS One. 2016;11: 1–19. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155442
Bieńkiewicz MMN, Bringoux L, Buloup F, Rodger M, Craig C, Bourdin C. The Limitations of Being a Copycat: Learning Golf Putting Through Auditory and Visual Guidance. Front Psychol. 2019;10: 1–19. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00092
Dyer JF, Stapleton P, Rodger M. Mapping sonification for perception and action in motor skill learning. Front Neurosci. 2017;11: 10–13. doi:10.3389/fnins.2017.00463
G W, C S. Principles derived form the studies of simple motor skills do not generalize to complex skill learning. Psychon Bull Rev. 2002;9: 185–211.
Wood G, Vine SJ, Wilson MR. The impact of visual illusions on perception, action planning, and motor performance. Attention, Perception, Psychophys. 2013;75: 830–834. doi:10.3758/s13414-013-0489-y
Chauvel G, Wulf G, Maquestiaux F. Visual illusions can facilitate sport skill learning. Psychon Bull Rev. 2015;22: 717–721. doi:10.3758/s13423-014-0744-9
Witt JK, Linkenauger SA, Proffitt DR. Get Me Out of This Slump! Visual Illusions Improve Sports Performance. Psychol Sci. 2012;23: 397–399. doi:10.1177/0956797611428810
Lewthwaite R, Wulf G. Optimizing motivation and attention for motor performance and learning. Curr Opin Psychol. 2017;16: 38–42. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.04.005
Palmer K, Chiviacowsky S, Wulf G. Enhanced expectancies facilitate golf putting. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2016;22: 229–232. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2015.08.009
Poolton JM, Masters RSW, Maxwell JP. The relationship between initial errorless learning conditions and subsequent performance. Hum Mov Sci. 2005;24: 362–378. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2005.06.006
Lam WK, Maxwell JP, Masters RSW. Probing the allocation of attention in implicit (motor) learning. J Sports Sci. 2010;28: 1543–1554. doi:10.1080/02640414.2010.517543
Fazeli D, Taheri H, Saberi Kakhki A. Random Versus Blocked Practice to Enhance Mental Representation in Golf Putting. Percept Mot Skills. 2017;124: 674–688. doi:10.1177/0031512517704106
Pascua LAM, Wulf G, Lewthwaite R. Additive benefits of external focus and enhanced performance expectancy for motor learning. J Sports Sci. 2015;33. doi:10.1080/02640414.2014.922693
Maxwell JP, Masters RSW, Eves FF. The role of working memory in motor learning and performance. Conscious Cogn. 2003;12: 376–402. doi:10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00005-9
Maxwell JP, Masters RSW, Eves FF. From novice to no know-how: A longitudinal study of implicit motor learning. J Sports Sci. 2000;18: 111–120. doi:10.1080/026404100365180
Ste-Marie DM, Vertes KA, Law B, Rymal AM. Learner-controlled self-observation is advantageous for motor skill acquisition. Front Psychol. 2013;3: 1–10. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00556
Tzetzis G, Lola AC. The effect of analogy, implicit, and explicit learning on anticipation in volleyball serving. Int J Sport Psychol. 2015;46: 152–166. doi:10.7352/IJSP2015.46.152
Tse ACY, van Ginneken WF. Children’s conscious control propensity moderates the role of attentional focus in motor skill acquisition. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2017;31: 35–39. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2017.03.015
Bonney E, Jelsma LD, Ferguson GD, Bouwien C. Learning better by repetition or variation ? Is transfer at odds with task specific training ? 2017; 1–17.
Roshandel S, Taheri H, Moghadam A, Behavior M, Behavior M. motor skill in children. 2017.
Wulf G. Attentional focus and motor learning: A review of 15 years. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol. 2013;6: 77–104. doi:10.1080/1750984X.2012.723728
Saemi E, Porter J, Wulf G, Ghotbi-Varzaneh A, Bakhtiari S. Adopting an external focus of attention facilitates motor learning in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Kinesiology. 2013;45: 179–185.
Wulf G, Lewthwaite R, Cardozo P, Chiviacowsky S. Triple play: Additive contributions of enhanced expectancies, autonomy support, and external attentional focus to motor learning. Q J Exp Psychol. 2017; 1–9. doi:10.1080/17470218.2016.1276204
Brady F. Contextual interference and teaching golf skills. Percept Mot Skills. 1997;84: 347–350. doi:10.2466/pms.1997.84.1.347
Bonney E, Jelsma LD, Ferguson GD, Smits-Engelsman BCM. Learning better by repetition or variation? Is transfer at odds with task specific training? PLoS One. 2017;12: 1–18. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0174214
Magill RA, Hall KG. A review of the contextual interference effect in motor skill acquisition. Hum Mov Sci. 1990;9: 241–289. doi:10.1016/0167-9457(90)90005-X
Rendell MA, Masters RSW, Farrow D, Morris T. An implicit basis for the retention benefits of random practice. J Mot Behav. 2011;43: 1–13. doi:10.1080/00222895.2010.530304
Malhotra N, Poolton JM, Wilson MR, Omuro S, Masters RSW. Dimensions of movement specific reinvestment in practice of a golf putting task. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2015;18: 1–8. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.11.008
Kal E, Prosée R, Winters M, Van Der Kamp J. Does implicit motor learning lead to greater automatization of motor skills compared to explicit motor learning? A systematic review. PLoS One. 2018;13: 1–25. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0203591
Abbas ZA, North JS. Good-vs. poor-trial feedback in motor learning: The role of self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation across levels of task difficulty. Learn Instr. 2018;55: 105–112. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.09.009
Wulf G, Iwatsuki T, Machin B, Kellogg J, Copeland C, Lewthwaite R. Lassoing Skill Through Learner Choice. J Mot Behav. 2018;50: 285–292. doi:10.1080/00222895.2017.1341378
Nourrit-Lucas D, Zelic G, Deschamps T, Hilpron M, Delignières D. Persistent coordination patterns in a complex task after 10 years delay. Subtitle: How validate the old saying “Once you have learned how to ride a bicycle, you never forget!” Hum Mov Sci. 2013;32: 1365–1378. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2013.07.005
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
Copyright (c) 2023 Revista Iberoamericana de Ciencias de la Actividad Física y el Deporte
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License.
All the contents published in Revista Iberoamericana de Ciencias de la Actividad Física y el Deporte are subject to the Creative Commons Reconocimento-NoComercia-Compartirigual 4.0 license, the full text of which can be found at <http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0>
They may be copied, used, disseminated, transmitted and publicly exposed, provided that:
The authorship and original source of your publication (Journal, editorial and URL of the work) are cited.
They are not used for commercial purposes.
The existence and specifications of this use license are mentioned.
Copyright is of two kinds: moral rights and patrimonial rights. Moral rights are perpetual, inalienable, inalienable, inalienable, inalienable and imprescriptible prerogatives.
In accordance with copyright legislation, Revista Eviterna recognizes and respects the moral rights of the authors, as well as the ownership of the economic right, which will be transferred to the University of Malaga for dissemination in open access.
The economic rights refer to the benefits obtained by the use or disclosure of the works. Revista Iberoamericana de Ciencias de la Actividad Física y el Deporte is published in open access and is exclusively authorized to carry out or authorize by any means the use, distribution, disclosure, reproduction, adaptation, translation or transformation of the work.
It is the responsibility of the authors to obtain the necessary permissions of the images that are subject to copyright.