Games may be older than culture and playing games in itself presuppose the existence of a society (Huzinga, 1955). A number of significant philosophers agree that games have been an important phenomenon in learning and teaching throughout human history (Sezgin, 2016). It can thus be claimed that game-playing is a very ancient aspect of society. According to Huzinga (1955), play is a voluntary activity, and universal. Suits (1967) defined a game as a voluntary activity designed to overcome inessential obstacles. Because games are voluntary activities, they may affect participants’ happiness, motivation, and creativity, and also increase achievement in an academic context. It has been pointed out that gamification, which is using game elements in non-gaming contexts, can enhance an individual’s experiences and sense of belonging (Deterding et al., 2011; Domínguez et al., 2013; Hanus, & Fox, 2015; Karataş, 2014; Kim, & Lee, 2015; Yıldırım, & Demir, 2014; Werbach, 2013). Challenges, rewards, levels, and point-scoring are examples of some “game elements” (Toda et al., 2019). It is important to make the learning process a journey; this can be done by using gamification to promote motivation and commitment in order to provide the conceptual understanding and ensure that learning experiences are deep and sustainable (Sezgin et al., 2018).
Previous studies of “gamification” in education focused on students’ motivation and attention in lessons (Abramovich et al., 2013; Alsawaier, 2018; Bayram, & Çalışkan, 2019; Bell, 2014; Harrold, 2015; Meşe, & Dursun, 2018). Although gamification uses game elements, Buckley and Doyle (2016) stated that gamification has negative effects when it is considered solely as a process that motivates students by using “rewards”. Similarly, some studies have shown that using “points” or “badges” has negative effects on students’ motivation (Hakulinen et al., 2015; Hanus, & Fox, 2015). Gamification, on the other hand, tends to adapt the system to the users’ desires, goals, and personalities (Bergmann et al., 2017). Players’ interests, willingness to participate, and opinions cannot be ignored. Santos et al. (2021) argued that the gamification design should be considered as user types. This is why game elements should be selected with reference to the target audience. In this way, any gamification in education should be adapted to student’s needs, goals, and characteristics because the target audience is students in a school environment. Since the players are students in a classroom, the students’ “player types” need to be determined in order to select appropriate game elements. However, the current player types scales are not able to fully represent the student’s profile in the class. If a player type scale is used to determine a player’s type with regard to game-playing, a learner profile scale should also be used to identify how students function in the school environment. The learning environment can thus be designed to meet the individual student’s needs, interests, and specific learner profile.
Differentiation is one of the most effective methods to meet students’ individual needs. Differentiated instruction is based on addressing students’ readiness to learn, interests, and learning profiles using differentiated content, products, or learning environments (Tomlinson, 2001). Differentiation consists of several components. In this study, “enrichment” was used as a component of differentiation. Enrichment can be expressed as an optional practice that can be used to prevent students from losing attention to the content they already know (Cutts, & Moseley, 2001). Thus, it was expected that it would have a positive effect on students in this study. It was considered that identifying appropriate game elements according to students’ needs and personalities and enriching lessons with these elements would have positive effects.
This study investigated the effect of mathematics activities enriched with game elements (MAEGEs) on middle-school students’ mathematics achievement, motivation, and attitudes towards mathematics lessons. It was necessary to know the students’ player types in order to select the appropriate game elements that would motivate them. However, since the player type scale (Andersen, & Downey, 2001) relate to online games and the questions are related to these kinds of games, the student’s responses to the questions may not have reflected their profile. For this reason, using any player type scale for games in Education was not considered sufficient to determine the students’ player types for each participant in education. For this reason, the Learner Profile Scale (Galiç, & Yıldız, 2020) was used to determine the students’ player types in the school environment. Learner Profile Scale determines students’ player types without using the player types scale in order to prepare gamification activities according to the target audience’s player types. Learner Profile Scale can be used in gamification applications in education during the design of the activities. Also, it can be used to determine the students’ profiles to identify them or follow their academic development. The Learner Profile Scale is associated with Bartle’s player types:
There are several game elements to motivate players according to their player type such as collecting, power, research, levels for achievers; imagination, learning, and completing for explorers; collaboration, communication, role-playing for socializers and competitions, challenges, and strategy for killers.
The purpose of the study was to investigate the effects of MAEGEs selected with respect to students’ player types as identified by the Learner Profile Scale on the student’s achievement, motivation, and attitudes toward mathematics lessons. The following three research questions were answered in the study:
This current study used the quantitative quasi-experimental approach. The matching-only design group was used from among the quasi-experimental designs because of non-randomization. In this design “the researcher still matches the subjects in both groups on certain variables’’ (Fraenkel, & Wallen, 2003). One of the researchers was the mathematics teacher of the experimental group. Another class from the same school was assigned as the control group because the students were at the same academic level as the experimental group. Each group was formed of 6th-grade students. The path of the study is given in Figure 1.
FIGURE 1. Path of the study
Although there was no randomization, the Academic Level Determination Test (ALDT) was performed with both groups to ensure that the mathematics academic level of the groups was equal. For this purpose, the ALDT was analyzed using the Mann-Whitney U Test. The result of the ALDT analysis is given in Table 1.
TABLE 1. Mann-Whitney U Test Analysis of Academic Level Determination Test
Groups | Measurement | N | X | U | z | p |
Experimental Group | ALDT | 12 | 14.08 | 53.000 | -1.108 | .268 |
Control Group | 12 | 10.92 |
As seen in Table 1, there was no significant difference between averages of the experimental (14.08) and control group (10.92) (p(24) = .268; p> 0.05). This result proved that there was no difference between groups before the implementation according to their academic level in mathematics. Thus, nonrandomization did not affect the implementation and the results of this study. To determine the appropriate game elements, the learner profiles of the students in the experimental group needed to be identified. The Learner Profile Scale was therefore applied to the students (Galiç, & Yıldız, 2020). The experimental group was found to be made up of students with the following player types: 40 % “Killers”, 30 % “Achievers”, 20 % “Socializers”, and 10 % “Explorers”. MAEGEs were designed according to the results of the scale. Two of these activities are given in Table 2.
TABLE 2. Two Examples of Mathematics Activities during Implementations
Activity | Learning Outcomes | Game Elements | Duration |
Relay Race | Operations with Fractions | Collaboration Competition | 20 min |
Where is the Place? | Operations with Fractions | Challenges Mission | 40 min |
When the age of the students in this study was considered, it was felt that applying all pre-tests on the same day may affect the results. Therefore, the pre-tests were planned for different days, but the same tests were applied to both groups on the same day. The Mathematics Motivation Scale (Aktan, & Tezci, 2013) and the Students’ Attitude Towards Mathematics Scale (Önal, 2013) was first applied on the same day to both groups, while the Academic Achievement Test was applied on the next day. After the pre-test, MAEGEs was implemented for two weeks in the experimental group. Each activity was about a fraction unit. The post-test was applied after the implementation.
Ethical approval for this study was obtained from Hacettepe University Ethics Committee (35853172-300 on 21.01.2020).
Twenty-four 6th-grade students participated in this study. The participants are shown in Table 3.
TABLE 3. Participants
Group / Gender | Female | Male | Total |
Experimental Group | 5 | 7 | 12 |
Control Group | 9 | 3 | 12 |
Total | 14 | 10 | 24 |
The Learner Profile Scale (Galiç, & Yıldız, 2020), the Academic Achievement Test, the Academic Level Determination Test (ALDT), the Mathematics Motivation Scale (Aktan, & Tezci, 2013), and the Students’ Attitudes Towards Mathematics Scale (Önal, 2013) were used to collect quantitative data. To determine whether there had been any change or not, these scales were applied before and after the implementation of MAEGEs.
The ALDT was designed by the researchers to evaluate the academic level of the experimental and control group before implementation. Each question was selected as related to topics in the units. Students had previously taken in the semester before they began the unit on fractions. The test included 10 questions designed with reference to the related literature. These questions were chosen from sample questions in national and international exams in order to ensure reliability and validity. Each question scored 10 for the correct answer and 0 for the wrong answer. The highest obtainable score for the scale was 100, while the lowest was 0.
The aim of this scale, which was developed by Galiç and Yıldız (2020), is to identify the learner profile of any person or group. It has 35 items on a 5-point Likert-type scale. The Learner Profile Scale identifies seven specific profiles: reflector (six items), inquirer (five items), collaborator (three items), researcher (four items), problem-solver (four items), and self-confidence (two items). The score for each item is between 1 and 5. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the scale was .912.
To assess the student’s academic achievement with regard to the “fractions’’ unit, the Academic Achievement Test was developed by researchers. The Academic Achievement Test was applied to the control and experimental groups before and after the implementation of MAEGEs. The test included 10 questions designed with reference to the related literature. These questions were chosen from sample questions in national and international exams in order to ensure reliability and validity. Each question scored 10 for the correct answer and 0 for the wrong answer. The highest obtainable score for the scale was 100, while the lowest was 0.
The aim of this scale, which was developed by Aktan and Tezci (2013), is to determine middle-school students’ motivation toward mathematics. It has 27 items and is a 5-point Likert-type scale. The internal consistency of this scale is between .84 and .94 and the item-total correlation is between .62 and .89.
The aim of this scale, which was developed by Önal (2013), is to determine students’ attitudes toward mathematics This scale consists of four factors such as interest, anxiety, study, and necessity. It has 22 items in a form of a five-point Likert scale. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was .90. Items are pointed between 1 and 5. Thus the score on this scale can get the highest 110 and the lowest 22 points.
Since the number of participants is lower than 30, the assumptions of normal distribution was not provided. Therefore, Mann-Whitney U test is used to analyse the non-normal continuous data for the differences between two independent groups At the beginning of the implementation, the Mann-Whitney U test was applied to the ADLT to observe the differences between the groups. The Mann-Whitney U test is used when the researcher wishes to analyze ranked data for nonparametric tests (Fraenkel, & Wallen, 2003). The effect size shows the size of the differences between groups to comprehend the result (Cohen, 1992). Hence, the effect size is calculated for each problem question. According to Cohen (1992), the criteria for effect size is given as following. If effect size is:
Before the implementation, the Academic Achievement Test, Mathematics Motivation Scale, and Students’ Attitude Towards Mathematics Scale were applied to each group. The differences between pre- and post-test should be important to analyze in both experimental and control group. The Wilcoxon signed-rank test is used since the data for pre- and post-test was from dependent samples which is defined ranoomly and data are the non-normal continuous . The same process was repeated for the control group and experimental group individually. Data were tested at a .05 level of significance in this study.
This study investigated the effect of MAEGEs on students’ achievement, attitudes, and motivation in mathematics lessons. In this section findings related to each research, and the problem is presented.
To assess the students’ prerequisite knowledge and readiness to learn, the Academic Achievement Test was applied. The Mann-Whitney U test was used to analyze data. No significant difference between the groups was observed in the ALDT. The results are given in Table 4.
TABLE 4. Mann-Whitney U Test Analysis of Pre-test of Academic Achievement
Groups | Measurement | N | X | U | z | p |
Experimental Group | AAT Pre-test | 12 | 12.00 | 78.000 | .353 | .724 |
Control Group | 12 | 13.00 |
As seen in Table 4, there was no significant difference between the average scores of the experimental group (X=12.00) and the control group (X=13.00) (p(24)=.724; p>0.05). According to this result, the student’s academic achievement in both groups was equal. Therefore, it can be argued that any future differences between groups were likely to relate to the implementation.
The Academic Achievement Test was given to both groups after implementation to examine the effect of the MAEGEs on the student’s achievement in mathematics. The result of the Wilcoxon signed-rank test for each group is given in Table 5.
TABLE 5. Wilcoxon Signed-rank Test Analysis of the Academic Achievement Test (AAT)
Groups | Measurement | N | X | Sd | p |
Experimental | AAT Pre-Test | 12 | 50.83 | 17,30 | .004 |
AAT Post-Test | 12 | 81.67 | 16,42 | ||
Control | AAT Pre-Test | 12 | 54.17 | 20,66 | .016 |
AAT Post-Test | 12 | 74.17 | 15,05 |
In Table 5 the average scores for the Academic Achievement Test which was performed with both groups are given as both pre-and post-test. There was a significant difference between the pre-test (X=50.83) and the post-test (X=81.67) in the average scores of the experimental group (p(12)=.004; p<0.05). Similarly, there was a significant difference between the pre-test (X=54.17) and post-test (74.17) in the control group (p(12) = .016; p<0.05). This result shows that the MAEGEs and lecture-based mathematics lessons contributed significantly to the student’s academic achievement. Therefore, to answer the first research question: MAEGEs positively affected the student’s academic achievement in mathematics.
Table 6 shows the results of data analysis to examine the difference between both groups with regard to the student’s academic achievement.
TABLE 6. Mann-Whitney U Test analysis of Post-test of Academic Achievement Test (AAT)
Groups | Measurement | N | X | U | Z | p |
Experimental Group | AAT Post Test | 12 | 14.21 | 51.500 | -1.202 | .229 |
Control Group | 12 | 10.79 |
Table 6 shows the results of students’ academic achievement after the implementation of the MAEGEs and lecture-based mathematics lessons. According to Table 5, there was a difference between the average of the experimental group (X=14.21) and the control group (X=10.79) yet this difference was not significant (p(24)= .229; p>0.05). This result shows that the MAEGEs contributed to students’ academic achievement at least in the lecture-based mathematics lessons. Since the effect size is calculated as 0.34, MAEGES can be interpreted as have a medium effect on student’s academic success.
To determine students’ motivation toward mathematics lessons in experimental and control groups, the Mathematical Motivation Scale was applied before the implementation. The results are given in Table 7.
TABLE 7. Mann-Whitney U Test analysis of the Pre-test of Mathematical Motivation Scale (MMS)
Groups | Measurement | N | X | U | z | p |
Experimental Group | MMS Pre-test | 12 | 14.12 | 52.500 | -1.129 | .259 |
Control Group | 12 | 10.88 |
As seen in Table 7, there was no significant difference between the averages of the experimental group (X=14.12) and the control group (X=10.88) (p(24)=.259; p>0.05). According to this result, the student’s motivation in both groups was equal. Therefore, it can be argued that any possible differences between groups would be related to the implementation.
The Mathematical Motivation Scale was applied to the experimental and control group after implementation to examine the effect of the MAEGEs on the student’s motivation toward mathematics lessons. The result of the Wilcoxon signed-rank test is given in Table 8.
TABLE 8. Wilcoxon Signed-rank Test Analysis of Mathematical Motivation Scale
Groups | Measurement | N | X | Sd | p |
Experimental Group | MMS Pre-test | 12 | 55.67 | 8.24 | .002 |
MMS Post-test | 12 | 107.92 | 11.26 | ||
Control Group | MMS Pre-test | 12 | 53.17 | 5.37 | .002 |
MMS Post-test | 12 | 99.50 | 10.01 |
Table 8 shows the averages for the Mathematical Motivation Scale both before and after implementation. There was a significant difference between the pre-test (X= 55.67) and post-test (X=107.92) in the averages of experimental group (p(12)= .002; p<0.05). Similarly, there was a significant difference between the pre-test (X=53.17) and post-test (X=99.50) in the control group (p(12)= .002; p<0.05). This result shows that MAEGEs and lecture-based mathematics lessons contributed significantly to students’ motivation toward mathematics lessons.
Table 9 shows the result of data analysis to investigate the difference between groups in terms of students’ motivation toward mathematics lessons after the implementation.
TABLE 9. Mann-Whitney U Test Analysis of Post-test of Mathematical Motivation Scale (MMS)
Groups | Measurement | N | X | U | z | p |
Experimental Group | MMS Post-test | 12 | 15.42 | 37.000 | -2.026 | .043 |
Control Group | 12 | 9.58 |
As seen in Table 9, there was a significant difference between the averages of the experimental group (X=15.42) and the control group (X=9.58) (p(24) = .043; p<0.05). This result shows that MAEGEs contributed to students’ motivation toward mathematics lessons more than lecture-based mathematics lessons. Since the effect size is calculated as 0.58, MAEGES can be interpreted as have a large effect on student’s motivation.
To determine the students’ attitudes towards mathematics lessons, the Students’ Attitudes Towards Mathematics Scale was applied to both groups before the implementation. The results are given in Table 10.
TABLE 10. Mann-Whitney U Test Analysis of Pre-test of Students’ Attitudes Towards Mathematics Scale (SATMS)
Groups | Measurement | N | X | U | z | p |
Experimental Group | SATMS Pre-test | 12 | 13.42 | 61.000 | -.635 | .525 |
Control Group | 12 | 11.58 |
As seen in Table 10, there was no significant difference between the average of the experimental group (X=13.42) and the control group (X=11.58) (p(24)= .525; p> 0.05). According to this result, the experimental and control groups were equal with regard to the students’ attitudes towards mathematics lessons. Thus, it can be argued that any possible differences between the groups after the application of MAEGEs would be related to the implementation.
The Students’ Attitudes Towards Mathematics Scale has applied again to the experimental and control groups after the implementation to examine the effect of the MAEGEs on the students’ attitudes towards mathematics lessons. Table 11 shows the results of the Wilcoxon signed-rank test for each group.
TABLE 11. Wilcoxon Signed-rank Test Analysis of Students’ Attitudes Towards Mathematics Scale (SATMS)
Groups | Measurement | N | X | Sd | P |
Experimental Group | SATMS Pre-test | 12 | 48.33 | 12.42 | .003 |
SATMS Post-test | 12 | 88.64 | 15.74 | ||
Control Group | SATMS Pre-test | 12 | 44.42 | 11.48 | .002 |
SATMS Post-test | 12 | 78.23 | 14.28 |
Table 11 presents the averages for the Students’ Attitudes Towards Mathematics Scale both before and after the implementation. According to the results, there was a significant difference between the averages of the experimental group pre-test (X= 48.33) and post-test (X=88.64) (p(12)= .003; p<0.05). Similarly, there was a significant difference between the pre-test (X=44.42) and post-test (X=78.23) in the control group (p(12)=.002; p<0.05). This result shows that MAEGEs and lecture-based mathematics lessons contributed significantly to students’ attitudes towards mathematics lessons.
Table 12 shows the difference between experimental and control groups in terms of the student’s attitudes toward mathematics lessons after the implementation.
TABLE 12. Mann-Whitney U Test Analysis of Post-test of the Students’ Attitudes Towards Mathematics Scale (SATMS)
Groups | Measurement | N | X | U | z | p |
Experimental Group | SATMS Post-test | 12 | 15.42 | 37.000 | -2.026 | .043 |
Control Group | 12 | 9.58 |
As seen in Table 12, there was a significant difference between the averages of the experimental group (X=15.42) and control group (X=9.58) (p(24) = .043; p>0.05). This result shows that MAEGEs contributed to students’ attitudes towards mathematics lessons more than lecture-based mathematics lessons. Since the effect size is calculated as 0.58, MAEGES can be interpreted as have a large effect on student’s attitudes.
This study investigated the effect of MAEGEs on students’ academic achievement, motivation, and attitude towards mathematics lessons. Understanding the student’s “player types” was necessary to determine the appropriate game elements for the experimental group. Since the game environment was the classroom, and the players were students in the school, it was necessary to determine the learner profiles of the students. After the player types had been determined using their Learner Profile Scale (Galiç, & Yıldız, 2020) without using any player type scale, the game elements required for the experimental group were then identified and the MAEGEs were designed. These activities were applied to the 6th-grade students as they studied fractions for a period of two weeks. Instruments were applied to both groups before and after the implementation in order to observe the effect of the MAEGEs on academic achievement, attitudes, and motivation.
The results of this study show that MAEGEs and lecture-based mathematics lessons contributed significantly to students’ academic achievement significantly. MAEGEs had a positive effect on the students’ mathematics academic achievement. This result is consistent with other studies on gamification (Bal, 2019; Chen et al., 2018; Harrold, 2015; Türkan, 2019; Welbers et al., 2019; Yürük, 2019). No difference was observed in the post-test Academic Achievement Tests for the experimental and control groups. This result shows that MAEGEs contributed to students’ academic achievement at least in the lecture-based process. This result is consistent with the study by Samur (2015). According to the school’s “Differentiation Policy”, all lessons had to be differentiated. Nevertheless, the “Differentiation Policy” may have affected the results.
MAEGEs contribute to students’ motivation towards mathematics lessons more than lecture-based mathematics lessons. This result is consistent with the literature (Bayram, & Çalışkan, 2019; Bell, 2014; Chen et al., 2018; Domínguez et al.; 2013; Harrold, 2015; Karamert, & Kuyumcu Vardar, 2021; Samur, 2015; Şahin et al; 2017; Türkan, 2019). On the other hand, this result is not consistent with some research (Meşe, & Dursun, 2018; Polat 2014). Unlike in other studies, determining the player types of the students in the experimental group and selecting appropriate game elements is thought to have had a positive effect in this study. While some researchers (Hakulinen et al., 2015; Hanus, & Fox, 2015) claimed the negative effects of gamification, the effect size is found as a larger effect for students’ motivation and attitudes in this study. Hence, it can be claimed that if educator prepare the lesson according to the students’ player types, the gamification has a positive effect during the learning process. As Buckle and Doyle (2016) mentioned that gamification means more than using badges in education. Since gamification path depends on the students’ personalities (Bergmann et al., 2017), the students should be well-known before creating a gamification environment to select the appropriate game elements.
According the result of this study, the effect size of MAEGE’s has medium on students’ academic achievement. Marín Suelves et al. (2021) argued that video games provide students to develop their cognitive and creative aspects. Thus, the effect of MAEGE’s may be expected to increase as the extension of implementation duration.
The results of this study showed that MAEGEs contributed to students’ attitudes towards mathematics lessons more than lecture-based mathematics lessons. According to the results of studies by Bal (2019), Polat (2014), Türkan (2019), and Yürük (2019) gamification affects students’ attitudes positively. Hence the result in this study is consistent with previous research.
This study investigated the effect of MAEGEs chosen according to middle-school students’ learner profiles on their academic achievement in a “fractions unit”, as well as their motivation and attitudes towards mathematics lessons. The study found that MAEGEs positively affected the student’s academic achievement, motivation, and attitudes. Although the results indicated that students’ motivation and attitudes towards mathematics were better in the experimental group, no differences were observed between the groups’ academic achievements. It can be argued that these activities were more effective than lecture-based mathematics lessons in the student’s motivation and attitudes toward mathematics lessons. The MAEGEs was implemented for a period of two weeks. Increasing the duration of implementation may also have a positive effect on students’ academic achievement, motivation, or attitudes in mathematics lessons. It can be also stated that MAEGEs designed according to students’ learner profiles positively affect students’ academic achievement, attitudes, and motivation in mathematics lessons. Santos et al. (2021) recommended that the gamification design should be personalized to obtain better result since the differences between different studies about gamification is related to the gamification design. In this study, educational gamification environment adapted to the students and the result shows that gamification has positive effects on students. In addition, it can be argued that such enrichment activities can be used not only for gifted students but also for all students in a mixed-ability classroom.
The following recommendations for future research can be made: the effects of using game elements with respect to students’ player types could be researched for different topics, different disciplines, or students of different grades. Game elements could also be used in differentiation instructions.
Abramovich, S., Schunn, C., & Higashi, R.M. (2013). Are badges useful in education?: It depends upon the type of badge and expertise of learner. Educational Technology Research and Development, 61, 217-232. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-013-9289-2
Aktan, S., & Tezci, E. (2013). The mathematical motivation scale (MMS) study of reliability and validity. The Journal of Academic Social Science Studies, 6(4), 57-77. https://doi.org/10.9761/JASSS1173
Alsawaier, R. S. (2018). The effect of gamification on motivation and engagement. The International Journal of Information and Learning Technology, 35(1), 56-79. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJILT-02-2017-0009
Andersen, E., & Downey, B. (2001). The MUD personality test. The MUD Companion, 1, 33–35. https://web.archive.org/web/20000818064001/http://www.andreasen.org/bartle/stats.cgi
Bal, M. (2019). Use of digital games in writing education: an action research on gamification. Comtemporary Educational Technology, 10(3), 246-271. https://doi.org/10.30935/cet.590005
Bayram, Y. T., & Çalışkan, H. (2019). Gamified creative activities used in social studies courses: action research. Journal of Individual Differences in Education, 1(1), 30-49. https://dergipark.org.tr/tr/download/article-file/724822
Bell, K. R. (2014). Online 3.0-The rise of the gamer educator the potential role of gamification in online education [Doctorate Thesis, University of Pennsylvania]. UPENN. https://repository.upenn.edu/dissertations/AAI3635727
Bergmann, N., Schacht, S., Gnewuch, U., & Maedche, A. (Eds.). (2017). Proceedings of the 38th International Conference on Information Systems (ICIS). Karlsruhe Institute of Technology.
Buckley, P., & Doyle, E. (2016). Gamification and student motivation. Interactive Learning Environments, 24(6), 1162-1175. https://doi.org/10.1080/10494820.2014.964263
Chen, C.C., Huang, C., Gribbins, M., & Swan, K. (2018). Gamify online courses with tools built into your learning management system (LMS) to enhance self-determined and active learning. Online Learning, 22(3), 41-54. https://doi.org/10.24059/olj.v22i3.1466
Cohen, J. (1992). Statistical power analysis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1(3), 98-101.
Cutts N. E., & Moseley, N. (2004). Education of gifted and talented children (İsmail Ersevim.). Özgür Publications.
Deterding, S., Sicart, M., Nacke, L.E., O’Hara, K., & Dixon, D. (2011). Gamification. using game-design elements in non-gaming contexts. Proceedings of 2011 Annual Conference Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI EA’11). ACM Digital Library. https://dl.acm.org/doi/10.1145/1979742.1979575
Domínguez, A., Saenz-de-Navarrete, J., De-Marcos, L., Fernández-Sanz, L., Pagés, C., & Martínez-Herráiz, J. J. (2013). Gamifying learning experiences: practical implications and outcomes. Computers & Education, 63, 380-392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2012.12.020
Fraenkel, J. R., & Wallen, N. E. (2003). How to design and evaluate research in education. McGraw-Hill.
Galiç, S., & Yıldız, B. (2020, Nov.22). Development of Learner Profile Scale To Be Used To Identify Students’ Player Types. 2th International Conference on Science, Mathematics, Entrepreneurship and Technology Education, Virtual, Turkey. http://2020.fmgtegitimikongresi.com/dosyalar/files/ozetler2.pdf
Hakulinen, L., Auvinen, T., & Korhonen, A. (2015). The effect of achievement on students’ behavior: an empirical study in a university- level computer science course. International Journal of Emerging Technologies in Learning (iJET), 10(1), 18-29. https://doi.org/10.3991/ijet.v10i1.4221
Hanus, M. D., & Fox, J. (2015). Assessing the effects of gamification in the classroom: a longitudinal study on intrinsic motivation, social comparison, satisfaction, effort, and academic performance. Computers & Education, 80, 152-161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2014.08.019
Harrold, D. J. (2015). Game on: a qualitative case study on the effects of gamified curriculum design on student motivational learning habits [Doctorate Thesis, Robert Morris University]. ProQuest Dissertations and Theses.
Huzinga, J. (1955). Homo Ludens: A Study of the Play Element in Culture. The Beacon Press.
Karamert, Ö., & Kuyumcu Vardar, A. (2021). The effect of gamification on young mathematics learners’ achievements and attitudes. Journal of Educational Technology & Online Learning, 4(2), 96-114. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1307874.pdf
Karataş, E. (2014). Gamification in Education: Research Trends. Ahi Evran University Journal of Education Faculty, 15(2), 315-333. https://doi.org/10.24106/kefdergi.380982
Kim, J. T., & Lee, W. H. (2015). Dynamical model for gamification of learning (DMGL). Multimedia Tools and Applications, 74(19), 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11042-013-1612-8
Marín Suelves, D., Vidal Esteve, M. I., Donato, D., & Granados Saiz, J. (2021). Análisis del estado del arte sobre el uso de los videojuegos en Educación Infantil y Primaria. Innoeduca: international journal of technology and educational innovation. 7(2), 4-18. https://doi.org/10.24310/innoeduca.2021.v7i2.11541
Meşe, C., & Dursun, Ö. (2018). Effectiveness of gamification elements in blended learning environments. Turkish Online Journal of Distance Education, 20(3), 119-142. https://doi.org/10.17718/tojde.601914
Önal, N. (2013). Turkish adaptation study of attitudes towards mathematics in secondary school students. İlköğretim Online, 12(4), 938-948. https://doi.org/10.20860/ijoses.486630
Polat, Y. (2014). A case study: Gamification and its effect on motivation of learners of English. [Master Thesis, Çağ University]. Council of Higher Education Thesis Center. https://acikbilim.yok.gov.tr/handle/20.500.12812/71558
Şahin, Y., Karadağ, N., Bozkurt, A., Doğan, E., Kılınç, H., Uğur, S., Gümüş, S., Öztürk, A., & Güler, C., (2017). The use of gamification in distance education: A web-based gamified quiz application, Turkish Online Journal of Qualitative Inquiry, 8(4), 372-395. https://doi.org/10.17569/tojqi.329742
Santos, A.C.G., Oliveira, W., Hamari, J., Rodrigues, L., Toda, A. M., Palomino, P., & Isotani, S. (2021) The relationship between user types and gamification designs. User Model User-Adap Inter, 31, 907–940. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11257-021-09300-z
Samur, Y. (2015). Gamifying a Hybrid Graduate Course [Conference session]. In Global Learn Conference, FernUniversitat in Hagen, Berlin, Germany.
Sezgin, S. (2016, May). İnsan ve Oyun: Oyunların Dünü, Bugünü, Yarını. VIII. International Congress of Educational Research, Çanakkale Onsekiz Mart University, Çanakkale, Turkey. http://congress.eab.org.tr/2016/media/2016_ozet_ kitap.pdf
Sezgin, S., Bozkurt, A., Yılmaz, E., & Van Der Linden, N. (2018). Gamification, education and theoretical approaches: Motivation, engagement and sustainability in learning process. Mehmet Akif Ersoy Education Faculty, 0(45), 169-189. https://doi.org/10.21764/maeuefd.339909
Suits, B. (1967). What Is a Game? Philosophy of Science, 34(2), 148-156. https://doi.org/10.1086/288138
Toda, A. M., Klock, A. C., Oliveira, W., Palomino, P. T., Rodrigues, L., Shi, L., Bittencourt, I., Gasparini, I., Isotani, S., & Cristea, A. I. (2019). Analysing gamification elements in educational environments using an existing Gamification taxonomy. Smart Learning Environments, 6(1), 1-14. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40561-019-0106-1
Tomlinson, C. A. (2001). Differentiated instruction in the regular classroom. Understanding Our Gifted, 14(1), 3-6.
Türkan, A. (2019). The effect of gamification method on the academic achievements, motivations and attitudes of secondary school students [Master Thesis, Atatürk University]. Council of Higher Education Thesis Center.
Welbers, K., Konijn, E. A., Burgers, C., de Vaate, A. B., Eden, A., & Brugman, B. C. (2019). Gamification as a tool for engaging student learning: A field experiment with a gamified app. E-Learning and Digital Media, 16(2), 92-109. https://doi.org/10.1177/2042753018818342
Werbach, K. (2013). Gamification: “Gamification Design Framework”. Coursera.
Yıldırım İ., & Demir S. (2014). Gamification and education. International Journal of Human Sciences, 11(1), 655-670. https://doi.org/10.14687/ijhs.v11i1.2765
Yürük N. (2019). Edutainment: Using Kahoot! as a Review Activity in Foreign Language Classrooms. Journal of Educational Technology & Online Learning, 2(2), 89-101. https://doi.org/10.31681/jetol.557518